Is Psychotherapy Getting Better?: A Progress Report on the Science-And Art-Of Our Profession

Article excerpt

Is Psychotherapy Getting Better?

A Progress Report on the Science--and Art--of Our Profession

By Diane Cole

This article's title poses a challenging question, to be sure--one filled with ambiguity and open to multiple answers. Getting better than what? Getting better, in what way? Getting better, according to whom? And the real kicker: Can we get better--and how? But if these tough questions are to be asked, there would seem to be no more fitting occasion than this magazine's 30th anniversary and the opportunity it provides to reflect on an era in the field of psychotherapy during which systematic efforts to quantify and measure the key factors in the psychotherapeutic process received more attention than ever before.

In a sense, the story of how to assess the effectiveness of therapy and how it might be improved began in 1952, 30 years before the first issue of this magazine appeared. In a classic paper that year, outspoken behavior therapist Hans Eysenck, one of the field's leading provocateurs at that time, took on psychotherapy. A staunch believer in science, he'd later be the subject of bomb threats and publicly punched in the nose by a protestor for his controversial views on genetics and IQ differences. The paper that concerns us here, "The Effects of Psychotherapy: An Evaluation," asserted there was no proof that psychotherapy worked. On the contrary, he claimed that surveys showed that patients suffering from clinical neuroses improved after two years, whether or not they were treated by a psychotherapist.

"In the absence of agreement between fact and belief," he proclaimed, "there is urgent need for a decrease in the strength of belief, and for an increase in the number of facts available. Until such facts as may be discovered in a process of rigorous analysis support the prevalent belief in therapeutic effectiveness of psychological treatment, it seems premature to insist on the inclusion of training in such treatment in the curriculum of the clinical psychologist." Eysenck concluded that the shortcomings of data "highlight the necessity of properly planned and executed experimental studies into this important field."

The first blast had been fired, and it was up to the profession to answer the challenge. Whatever its origins in Freud's grand speculations and couch-based methodology, psychotherapy's modern quest for scientific legitimacy may be said to have begun here.

From the get-go, however, measurement issues loomed: How do you go about proving psychotherapy really is more effective than a placebo or more helpful than a friendly, sympathetic listener? How do you determine objective measures with which to identify, define, and quantify the variable, and sometimes intangible-seeming, factors and aspects that contribute to a successful course of treatment? Moreover, as you go about establishing the science of psychotherapy, what happens to the intuitive art of psychotherapy? Until recently, this divide between objective science and intuitive art characterizing the uneasy relationship between psychotherapy's researchers and practitioners has appeared unbridgeable.

A 2009 press release from the Association for Psychological Science, an organization formed by researchers who felt the American Psychological Association (APA) had become more of a guild than a scientific organization, carried the headline "Where's the science? The sorry state of psychotherapy." The release's lead paragraph reads like an indictment: "The prevalence of mental health disorders in this country has nearly doubled in the past 20 years. Who is treating all these patients? Clinical psychologists and therapists are charged with the task, but many are falling short by using methods that are out of date and lack scientific rigor." It goes on to cite a study suggesting that, in the absence of such scientific rigor, "six out of every seven sufferers were not getting the best care available from their clinicians. …