Biological Psychiatry - Vol. 2

By Hugo D'Haenen; J.A. Den Boer et al. | Go to book overview
Save to active project

XVIII-8
Functional Neuroscience of Mood Disorders

Michel Le Moal and Willy Mayo


INTRODUCTION

The evolution of criteria, symptoms and models for diagnosing mood disorders, as reflected in classical twentieth-century textbooks, has been a long process, if we consider Kraepelin's (1921) phenomenological characterizations to constitute a historical landmark. This is especially true for classification and the progressive importance of biological theories. A depressive state is considered to result from either endogenous determinants or a reaction to environmental or life events and a failure to adapt. The evolution from the first edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-I) (American Psychiatric Association, 1952) to the fourth edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) illustrates, through the push for more reliable and valid diagnoses and for a change to a categorical system (notably for research), profound changes due largely to the new pharmacological thought and its aminergic theories and derivatives. However, it remains clear mat the parallel neurobiological investigation on mood has not played a pivotal role, and that pathophysiological mechanisms have still not been identified, at least in a specific manner, in relation to the categories proposed by the last DSM (Boland and Keller, 1999). Moreover, the relation between the genetic aspects of susceptibility to mood disorders (mainly bipolar) and possible defects at the neuronal levels have not yet been demonstrated. These problems are now at the forefront of psychiatric research. Thanks to advances in statistical analysis combined with powerful new biotechnology methods, hypotheses can now be explored more rapidly. Replicated linkage has, at present, proposed susceptibility loci on chromosomes X, 18 and 21, but a single locus genetic model is in no way responsible for unipolar, bipolar and recurrent disorders (for review, see Sanders et al, 1999), leading to multifactorial vulnerabilities, including environmental factors as well as many genes. Finally, promising data from brain-imaging techniques have opened a new era to help identify circuitries involved in the physiopathology and proposed new neuroanatomies of mood disorders (Manji et al, 2001). However, these investigations raise methodological problems.


NEUROSCIENCE OF MOOD DISORDERS:
THEORETICAL AND METHODOLOGICAL
CONSDDERATIONS

Difficulties for an Experimental Psychopathology
of Mood Disorders

Basic and experimental neurosciences and clinical neuropsychiatry have joined forces over the past decade to promote the biological foundations of mental illness. The task is so difficult that it can be considered, together with the mind-consciousness problem, as the last frontier in biomedical research and the definitive inclusion of psychiatry in medical sciences. In spite of significant advances that have transformed the clinical perspectives in neurology and related disorders (obsessive-compulsive disorders and Tourette's syndrome are good examples), the growing tendency to 'neurologize' classic psychiatric syndromes, such as the various forms of schizophrenia, has not yet led to universal consensus regarding the aetiology and pathophysiology of such major mental illnesses.

From a neurobiological standpoint, and in the perspective of the neuroanatomical bases of the syndromes, three main general questions remain to be solved. First, when considering a given syndrome or disorder, or a specific mental disease, the symptoms that define the disease still have to be related to a given (or a set of related) 'normal' integrated functions. In other words, a continuum from physiology to pathology and vice versa is expected and, if so, then these functional dimensions should correlate with anatomical loci. The neurobiological interpretations of the symptoms have to be placed within the framework of the structure-function paradigm (Pearlson, 1999), in which modern neurosciences and neuropsychology have made enormous progress, even regarding mental functions and higher cognitive processes. The neuropsychological paradigm first applied to the neurology of cortical functions is now open to psychiatric symptoms. For instance, the disinhibition syndrome characteristic of acute mania will be referred to a similar syndrome sometimes seen after frontal brain injury, and mania will be attributed to a dysfunction of this region.

Second, by the same token, using working hypotheses inspired by experimental medicine, and considering that the concept of experimental psychopathology results directly from the structure-function paradigm, the neurobiologist could create appropriate animal models of the given illness by manipulating either the function or the underlying structure. In this context, the interpretation of experimental conditions is limited by the fact that most of the major human mental disorders do not exist per se in animals. Moreover, two other problems render experimental psychopathology difficult: on the one hand, manipulation of the model to obtain all the basic symptoms or dimensions that could characterize the illness or syndrome is theoretically impossible because they are known mainly by subjective report, and on the other hand, these basic symptoms may also be found in other heterogeneous disorders, because of the difficulty of defining mental illness, as reflected by ever changing concepts in psychiatry, or due to the clinical reality of considerable comorbidities. For instance, many depressed patients display anxiety symptoms, while anxiety and depression are also considered separately in DSM-IV.

Third, the pharmacological paradigm is not totally satisfying. It is based on a well-known working hypothesis: (1) the active drug acts on neuronal systems that are defective and part of the pathophysiology of the disease; (2) it is of major interest to discover the site of action of therapeutic drugs, and these sites or structures have generally been characterized as being the specific

-803-

Notes for this page

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this page

Cited page

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited page

Bookmark this page
Biological Psychiatry - Vol. 2
Table of contents

Table of contents

Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this book

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen
/ 730

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?