Biological Psychiatry - Vol. 2

By Hugo D'Haenen; J.A. Den Boer et al. | Go to book overview

XIX-1
Animal Models of Anxiety Disorders

Frederico G. Graeff and Hélio Zangrossi Jr


INTRODUCTION

The last decades have witnessed a growing interest in the use and development of animal models in psychiatry. The demand for these models has been motivated by the ethical, methodological and economical constraints that restrain the study of neurobiological processes in human subjects. It is undeniable that animal experimentation has contributed to advances in psychopharmacology and to our understanding of brain mechanisms involved in psychiatric disorders. Suffice to remember that benzodiazepine anxiolytics have been discovered because of the taming effect of chlordiazepoxide observed in Cynomolgus monkeys by Randall (Randall et al, 1960). Also, hypotheses on the role of serotonin (5-HT) in anxiety were based on experimental work with conflict tests in rats and pigeons (Robichaud and Sledge, 1969; Graeff and Schoenfeld, 1970; Stein, Wise and Berger, 1973) as well as with aversive electrical stimulation of the rat dorsal periaqueductal grey matter (DPAG) (Kiser and Lebovitz, 1975; Schenberg and Graeff, 1978, Schiitz, de Aguiar and Graeff, 1985).

Early animal models of anxiety were developed when behaviourism was the main conceptual framework within experimental psychology, and before classifications of psychiatric disorders split pathological anxiety into distinct nosological entities (see later). These animal models of anxiety rely on either inhibition of ongoing behaviour elicited by conditioned stimuli that predicted unavoidable electric shock or on suppression of rewarded responding by electric-shock punishment. The first type of model is based on principles of associative or Pavlovian conditioning, and the second on instrumental or operant conditioning. Punishment tests also suggest clinically derived constructs that emphasize the role of inner conflict in pathological anxiety, and this may be the reason why these tests became known as conflict models. Early pharmacological analysis showed conflict models to have a higher predictive value than conditioned suppression and, as a result, punishment tests became paradigmatic for assaying anti-anxiety drugs (Kelleher and Morse, 1968).

Surprisingly, classical conflict tests failed to consistently detect the anxiolytic action of drugs that act primarily on the neurotransmission mediated by serotonin (5-HT), such as buspirone and ritanserin (Handley et al., 1993; Griebel, 1995). Such falsenegative results undermined the general confidence in conflict models, although many arguments may be summoned to their defence, as for instance the time course of drug action. Unlike benzodiazepine anxiolytics, the newer drugs need several weeks of continuous administration to become clinically effective, initial doses being sometimes anxiogenic (Nutt, 1991). Why, men, should one expect single administration of these agents to be anxiolytic in animal models? In spite of this, it became generally accepted that conflict tests were good only for anxiolytics that acted primarily on the neurotransmission mediated by γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), which is the case for barbiturates and benzodiazepines.

A theoretical shift from behaviourism to ethology has also contributed to the discredit of conflict models, which have been criticized because of their artificiality and the confounding influence of appetitive drives, such as hunger and thirst, and of pain (Treit, 1985). As a result, a search for ethologically based animal models of anxiety has begun. The most widely used animal model of anxiety resulting from this trend has been the elevated 'X' or 'plus' maze, which is based on the natural fear that rats have for elevated, open spaces, represented by the two open arms of the apparatus (Handley and Mithani, 1984; Pelow and File, 1986). Coundess drugs have been assayed in this model, and the results obtained summarized in several comprehensive reviews (e.g., Handley et al., 1993; Rodgers, 1994; Griebel, 1995). Disappointingly, this model also failed to detect non-benzodiazepine anxiolytics. To do this, ethological analysis of behavioural items shown by the animals while exploring the elevated plus-maze has been added to the procedure (Cruz, Frei and Graeff, 1994; Rodgers and Johnson, 1995). However, with this modification one of the main advantages of the test, which is simplicity, is lost.

As these developments were taking place in basic research, the split of anxiety disorders into distinct diagnostic categories, a trend that was initiated by the DSM III classification of psychiatric disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), became accepted worldwide. Even though present-day psychiatric classifications cluster symptoms empirically, they constitute a necessary starting point for systematic research. It is hoped that the evidence thus obtained will either validate or modify the original categories.

The following categories from the DSM IV classification of psychiatric disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) will be considered in the present analysis. (1) Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), a state of excessive anxiety or apprehension lasting for more man six months. Neurovegetative symptoms are often present, but relatively minor. (2) Panic disorder (PD), characterized by recurrent panic attacks, either unexpected or associated with particular situations. Panic attacks are sudden surges of intense fear or terror, desire of fleeing and feeling of imminent death, going crazy or loosing control. These subjective symptoms are accompanied by major neurovegetative changes, such as palpitation, hypertension, dyspnoea, difficulty in deep breathing, sweating, urge to void the bladder and increased peristalsis. This leads to worry about the next attack or anticipatory anxiety, and avoidance of places where a panic attack would be embarrassing. Ultimately, generalized avoidance or agoraphobia may ensue. Nevertheless, agoraphobia sometimes occurs without panic attacks. (3) Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) characterized by intrusive, distressing thoughts (obsessions) and/or stereotyped or ritualized behaviour (compulsions) that must be performed in order to alleviate intense anxiety. (4) Specific phobias, which are irrational fears of either objects (animals, blood, pointed instruments) or situations (heights, closed environments). (5) Social

-879-

Notes for this page

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this book

This book has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this book

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this page

Cited page

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited page

Bookmark this page
Biological Psychiatry - Vol. 2
Table of contents

Table of contents

Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this book

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen
/ 730

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.