Examining the Persuasive Effect of Statistical Messages: A Test of Mediating Relationships

By Lindsey, Lisa L. Massi; Yun, Kimo Ah | Communication Studies, Fall 2003 | Go to article overview

Examining the Persuasive Effect of Statistical Messages: A Test of Mediating Relationships


Lindsey, Lisa L. Massi, Yun, Kimo Ah, Communication Studies


Since the beginning of the 20th century, persuasion researchers have been concerned with answering questions regarding the major determinants of message effectiveness (e.g., Boster & Mongeau, 1984; Chaiken, 1980; McCroskey, 1969; O'Keefe, 1990; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a; Stiff, 1986). Much of this research has explored the important components of the persuasion process by positing and testing research questions and hypotheses to reveal the conditions under which messages are most persuasive.

In their quest to extend the persuasion literature, researchers sought to address questions centered generally around knowing two things: what kind of persuasive message is most effective (e.g., Allen et al., 1990; Hale, Mongeau, & Thomas, 1991; McCroskey, Young, & Scott, 1972), and what factors contribute to the persuasiveness of a message (e.g., Arnold & McCroskey, 1967; Harte, 1976; McCroskey & Young, 1981; Petty & Cacioppo, 1984; Slater & Rouner, 1997). In order to answer these questions, the dominant research approach has been to vary the content of a persuasive message in experimental conditions and to examine subsequently its persuasive outcome. In doing this, scholars have examined systematically the persuasiveness of a variety of message types such as refutational (e.g., Allen et al., 1990; Hale et al., 1991; McCroskey et al., 1972; O'Keefe, 1998; Winkel, 1984), narrative (e.g., Ah Yun & Massi, 2001; Kazoleas, 1993; Kopfman, Smith, Ah Yun, & Hodges, 1998), and statistical appeals (e.g., Ah Yun & Massi 2000; Dickson, 1982; Yalch & Yalch, 1984).

The research focusing on persuasive messages has increased our understanding of various message effects in three important ways. First, we know that various message types successfully elicit the desired attitude (see, for example, McCroskey, 1967, 1969; Nadler, 1983; Reinard, 1988). With respect to refutational messages, for example, Allen et al. (1990) compared the persuasive effect of three different messages. One message presented only positive statements, the second presented a two-sided refutational message, and the third was a two-sided nonrefulational message. Allen and his colleagues found that the refutational message was more persuasive than the one-sided message, which, in turn, was more persuasive than the two-sided nonrefutational message. Research such as this has enabled scholars to determine the types of messages that are most persuasive and elicit desired attitudes.

Second, the research has provided explanations as to why various message types are persuasive (e.g., Harte, 1976; Rosenthal, 1971; Stiff, 1986). For example, in an examination of fear appeals, Witte (1992) developed a model to explicate the processes involved when individuals are exposed to fear appeals. Her model suggests that fear appeal messages that induce a perceived threat in a receiver will promote a positive health behavior change so long as that person believes that a behavioral change is possible and that it will successfully avert the threat presented in the message.

Finally, previous research has explained that any number of intervening factors may explain the effect of a given message. For example, Ah Yun and Massi (2001) found that sympathy, happiness, and perceived vividness mediated the relationship between content variations of a narrative message and a person's attitude toward signing an organ donor card.

This body of research suggests that understanding which types of messages are most persuasive, why specific messages are persuasive, and which factors mediate the relationship between the type of message and its outcomes are important components of understanding persuasive messages. Although a great deal of the persuasive message effects research has answered the above questions in regards to many specific message types-for example, refutational and narrative messages-scholars have not undertaken such an examination of statistical appeals to determine why they are persuasive and which factors mediate the relationship between statistical messages and their effects.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Examining the Persuasive Effect of Statistical Messages: A Test of Mediating Relationships
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.