Using Metacognitive Skills to Improve 3rd Graders' Math Problem Solving

By Goldberg, Patricia D.; Bush, William S. | Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, Fall 2003 | Go to article overview

Using Metacognitive Skills to Improve 3rd Graders' Math Problem Solving


Goldberg, Patricia D., Bush, William S., Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics


For the past twenty years, problem solving has been touted as a primary focus for mathematics instruction at all grade levels (National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics, 1978; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 1980, 1989, 2000; Mathematical Sciences Education Board, 1989). Despite this persistent call for problem-solving as an instructional approach, teachers across the United States struggle with helping children solve problems. National and international assessments consistently reveal that U.S. students do not perform well on problem-solving tasks that require more than one step (DosKey, 1993; Dossey, 1994; Kenney and Silver, 1997; Silver, 1998; Stigler and Hiebert, 1997). Furthermore, studies of mathematics teachers at all grade levels indicate that teachers have difficulty in planning and implementing lessons that build students' problem-solving skills. (Cooney, 1985; Silver, 1985; Thompson, 1989) Clearly, teachers need instructional strategies that help students become better problem solvers.

The literature on the teaching of problem solving over the past twenty years promotes building students' metacognitive skills--planning, monitoring, and evaluating ones own thinking--as a means to improve their problem-solving skills (Costa, 1991; Perkins, 1992, 1995; Fogarty, 1994; Marzano et al, 1997; Swartz and Parks, 1994; Tishman, Perkins, and Jay, 1995). As adults, we see some evidence of children planning, monitoring, and evaluating. Parents listen as their children explain what candy they will select at the movie theatre. Teachers hear children stop what they are doing in a group and tell another child he is not doing the assignment correctly. A grandparent may hear a grandchild comment confidently about the quality of a drawing he insists be displayed on the refrigerator. When given a novel task in school, however, children are very likely to jump into the problem with one strategy, continue the strategy without "looking back," and finish without reexamining the solution. Often, the result can be a misunderstood problem, or an ineffective strategy, and/or a solution that does not work. According to Perkins (1995), metacognition, or reflective intelligence as he calls it, "particularly supports coping with novelty" (p. 112). Perkins also suggests that reflective intelligence "supports thinking contrary to certain natural trends" (p. 113), thus contributing to breaking mental sets and exploring new ideas.

If students across the United States are to become proficient mathematical problem solvers, teachers at all grade levels must learn how to develop and assess metacognitive skills in their students. Teachers are searching for instructional strategies that will help students plan, monitor, and evaluate their own thinking during problem solving. This study seeks to determine the effectiveness of a set of year-long instructional strategies designed to improve metacognitive skills, thereby improving the problem-solving skills of eight- and nine-year old students.

Theoretical Basis

Metacognition emerged as an important mental activity for solving problems when researchers began to study children's intelligence and problem solving. Early analyses of problem-solving performance revealed that good or expert problem solvers tended to plan, monitor, and evaluate their thinking during problem solving more often and more efficiently than did poor or novice problem solvers (Flavell, 1976). More recent studies have confirmed this finding for students at middle school, high school, and college levels (Bookman, 1993; Cai, 1994; Lucangeli, Coi, and Bosco, 1997). Flavell (1976) defined metacognition broadly as "one's knowledge concerning one's own cognitive processes and products ... [and] the active monitoring and consequent regulation and orchestration of these processes" (p. 232).

Based on this definition, Garofalo and Lester (1985) identified three types of metacognitive knowledge related to mathematical problem solving: person knowledge; task knowledge; and strategy knowledge. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Using Metacognitive Skills to Improve 3rd Graders' Math Problem Solving
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.