Partitioning Visual Displays Aids Task-Directed Visual Search

By Haimson, Craig; Bothell, Daniel et al. | Human Factors, Fall 2004 | Go to article overview

Partitioning Visual Displays Aids Task-Directed Visual Search


Haimson, Craig, Bothell, Daniel, Douglass, Scott A., Anderson, John R., Human Factors


INTRODUCTION

Processing a visual display often requires a search for a target symbol embedded within a field of distractor symbols. There is still considerable disagreement as to why the difficulty of visual search increases as the similarity of targets and distractors increases (e.g., Duncan & Humphreys, 1989; Treisman, 1993; Wolfe, 1996). However, there is some consensus that only a limited amount of information can be fully analyzed at a given time in displays with relatively low signal-to-noise ratios. Finding a target symbol in such a display generally requires some amount of item-by-item or region-by-region processing, with observers repeatedly shifting the location of eye fixation and attentional focus to different locations in the display until the currently analyzed region contains the target and the perceptual representation of this signal surpasses some threshold level of activation.

Laboratory visual search paradigms generally entail the presentation of targets in random locations within experimental displays that may be searched in whatever manner the observer chooses. Of course, the perceptual organization of such displays may encourage a certain pattern in the sequence of ocular/attentional fixations, or scanpath (e.g., circular displays encourage circular sequences, blocks of text encourage left-to-right horizontal sequences). However, there is generally no principled reason for choosing a starting point in such tasks, and observers may often follow a roughly random scanpath for such searches (Scinto, Pillalamarri, & Karsh, 1986). In contrast, real-world visual search tasks often impose additional constraints on the scanning process. Locating a target symbol on a radar screen is one instance of a real-world search in which observers generally adopt a nonrandom scanning procedure; operators generally assess the composition of tracks in the display with specific information-seeking goals in mind (e.g., "How close is the target symbol to Position X?"). Finding a target in one region of the display may be more important than finding it in another region. It is this form of strategic "task-directed" search that we sought to understand better in the current set of experiments.

Following a prescribed scanpath shares many similarities with spatial precuing. Preexisting knowledge about the probable spatial locations in which target information will appear greatly aids visual processing. Numerous studies have demonstrated that participants' responses to probe stimuli are quicker and more accurate when the stimuli are presented at or near cued locations (e.g., Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980). This "cue validity effect" (so called because enhancement occurs when cues validly predict target location) is generally attributed to the allocation of spatial attention to the cued area (Posner et al., 1980). Providing observers with a prespecified order in which to attend to different regions in a display should, therefore, have the same consequences as indicating those areas with spatial precues.

If following a prescribed scanpath ("Find target closest to Point X") encourages a sequence of ocular/attentional fixations that mimic precuing, then its effects may be enhanced by the addition of perceptual boundaries that delineate to-be-attended regions in the display. Although observers may be capable of confining their attention to an area of less than a visual degree under the right conditions (Nakayama & Mackeben, 1989), they typically experience considerable difficulty restricting attention to an unbounded region in a display. For example, observers generally find it challenging to respond to target stimuli flanked by distractors associated with different responses (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). This difficulty may arise partially because observers tend to focus their attention on entire perceptual objects (Duncan, 1984), and similar-looking target and distractor stimuli can appear to form a single perceptual group that encourages the allocation of such "object-based" attention (Baylis & Driver, 1992). …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Partitioning Visual Displays Aids Task-Directed Visual Search
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.