Safety Symbol Comprehension: Effects of Symbol Type, Familiarity, and Age

By Hancock, Holly E.; Rogers, Wendy A. et al. | Human Factors, Summer 2004 | Go to article overview

Safety Symbol Comprehension: Effects of Symbol Type, Familiarity, and Age


Hancock, Holly E., Rogers, Wendy A., Schroeder, Derek, Fisk, Arthur D., Human Factors


INTRODUCTION

Pictorial symbols may be a valuable form of communication for conveying safety information. Symbols can enhance text comprehension, facilitate top-down processing of a safety message, be better perceived than text in degraded conditions, and be more rapidly perceived than text (for a review, see Rogers, Lamson, & Rousseau, 2000). Symbols may also be useful in conveying safety information to a diverse audience of consumers and product users. For example, people with low of impaired vision might perceive and comprehend symbols more readily than information presented via text. Although people with low literacy levels may have difficulty understanding different types of safety and warning messages if detailed text is used, they may have higher comprehension for symbolic information. Furthermore, non-native-English speakers need not rely on their knowledge of the English language to correctly interpret symbols.

For older individuals, comprehension of safety information presented in symbolic form may also be better than when the same information is presented in textual form. The typical older adult may experience visual, memory, and text comprehension impairments (e.g., Craik & Salthouse, 1992), all of which could contribute to difficulties comprehending text messages. Furthermore, besides possibly being exposed to safety symbols at work, there is evidence that older adults continue to use a variety of household products that may display, symbols conveying usage and safely information (Hancock, Fisk, & Rogers, 2001). Thus it is important that these individuals be able to interpret symbols in the way, designers intended them to be interpreted.

Unfortunately, the existing empirical evidence is mixed with regard to older adults' comprehension of symbols, For example, researchers assessing comprehension for highway icon signs typically have found no significant age-related differences for symbol comprehension by' younger and older drivers (e.g.. Halpern, 1984; Kline & Fuchs, 1993: Kline, Ghali, Kline, & Brown, 1990). However, other research involving symbols used in railway stations (Zwaga & Boersma, 1985), on medication bottles (Morrell, Park, & Peon, 1990), and on consumer products (Easterby & Hakiel, 1981: Hancock, Rogers, & Fisk, 2001) has found evidence for an age-related deficit in comprehension. To further complicate the issue, at least one study has reported that older adults have greater comprehension for certain symbols than do younger adults (Mayer & Laux, 1989). It is important to understand how people of different ages process this type of warning information, and a lot Call be learned from studies of symbol comprehension about how age affects specific cognitive processes in a real-world problem space. Findings from this kind of empirical research can be applied toward improving warning presentation and design as well as toward furthering a theoretical understanding of principles underlying cognitive aging.

Differences in the testing methods used by researchers may have contributed to the mixed results observed across studies. For example, open-ended, multiple-choice, or ranking tests may be used to assess comprehensibility of symbols. Specific methodologies for designing and evaluating safety symbols have been asserted by the American National Standards Institute (ANSI: National Electrical Manufacturers Association, 1991, 1998) as a means of standardizing the way symbols are developed. The standards recommended iterative testing, using multiple candidates for the same concept, using multiple tests, and testing appropriate user populations. ANSI dictated that a given symbol should be associated with an 85% correct response rate for it to be considered effective (i.e., reducing reliance on text messages and symbol training). A number of studies, however, have found common ANSI symbols to be associated with accuracy rates below 83% (e. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Safety Symbol Comprehension: Effects of Symbol Type, Familiarity, and Age
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.