Impact of Seat Belt Use on Driving Behavior

By Singh, Harinder; Thayer, Mark | Economic Inquiry, October 1992 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

Impact of Seat Belt Use on Driving Behavior


Singh, Harinder, Thayer, Mark, Economic Inquiry


I. INTRODUCTION

The National Safety Council estimates that the expected reduction in occupant death rates due to usage of lap seat belts would be 7-8.5 percent (see Peltzman |1975~). In addition, Campbell and Campbell |1988~ estimate that fatalities in the twenty-five states with seat belt laws were 6.6 percent lower than forecast for these states. This improvement amounts to approximately 1,300 lives saved. Swan |1984~ reports that seat belts are negatively correlated with the number of traffic fatalities. This conclusion is based on a pooled cross-section, time-series model for Australia and New Zealand. Finally, McEwin |1986~ estimates on the basis of his empirical model that a 100 percent usage of seat belts would reduce fatalities by 40 percent.

However, a number of studies have pointed out that, contrary to conventional wisdom, seat belt laws may not reduce the total number of fatalities. Their argument is that use of the safety belt provides the driver with an additional sense of security which translates into relatively more reckless driving. There is a potential for offsetting or compensating behavior on the part of the driver of the automobile. This notion is referred to as the "compensating-behavior hypothesis."

Compensating behavior is justified on both theoretical and empirical grounds. Blomquist |1986~ finds that compensating behavior is utility maximizing if an individual's safety effort and exogenous safety measures (e.g., government regulations) are substitutes in determining the probability of and loss from an automobile accident.

Peltzman |1975; 1976~ tested the impact of automobile safety regulation on both occupant and non-occupant deaths. He found evidence from aggregate time-series and cross-section data that occupants' lives are saved at the expense of pedestrian deaths and a larger number of nonfatal accidents. Recently, Garbacz |1991a~ tested this hypothesis using aggregate time-series data for New Zealand. He found that a mandatory seat belt law, enacted in 1972, was negatively correlated with the deaths of automobile occupants and positively associated with deaths of cyclists and pedestrians. In fact, the Garbacz study indicated complete offsetting in that total deaths showed no relationship to seat belt use (savings in occupant deaths were just offset by the increase in non-occupant deaths). In other work Garbacz |1990; 1991b; 1992~ provides further evidence of offsetting behavior, demonstrating that both non-occupants and rear-seat passengers are more at risk when seat belt laws are in effect. A final example of offsetting behavior was provided in a recent Wall Street Journal article (10 October 1991) that showed that air bags seem to reduce fatalities, but accidents and other associated injuries have increased.

One concern in the Garbacz |1991a~ work was the sensitivity of the results to the "speed variable" (average open-road speed for the 85th percentile of vehicles). He concluded that this variable could be endogenous to the estimation process. Thus, the compensating-behavior hypothesis would imply more risk taking (including speeding) after the imposition of the seat belt law. In fact, Lave and Weber |1970~ first suggested the possibility that mandated safety devices might lead to faster driving, offsetting some or all of the beneficial effect of the safety device.

In this paper we re-examine the issue of compensating behavior when individuals use seat belts. Our investigation has a number of significant departures from previous studies. First, since compensating behavior is a hypothesis of individual action, we test the hypothesis using individual-specific observations. The studies discussed above employed aggregate time-series or cross-section data. At the aggregate level, the statistical regularities could conceivably be caused by other confounding factors.

Second, we test the compensating-behavior hypothesis after accounting for tastes in risks (as manifested by precautionary steps individuals have taken to reduce everyday risks).

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

Impact of Seat Belt Use on Driving Behavior
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?