Development of Reasoning about Random Events

By Watson, Jane M.; Caney, Annaliese | Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, Fall 2005 | Go to article overview

Development of Reasoning about Random Events


Watson, Jane M., Caney, Annaliese, Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics


Abstract

The development of school students' understanding of random events is explored in three related studies based on tasks well known in the research literature. In Study 1,99 students in Grades 3 to 9 were interviewed on three tasks and surveyed on two tasks about luck and random behaviors in chance settings. Four levels of response are identified across the five tasks, reflecting increasing structural complexity and statistical appropriateness. In Study 2,23 of these students were interviewed on the same interview protocol tasks, three or four years later to monitor developmental change. In Study 3, a different group of 15 students was interviewed with two of the tasks and prompted with conflicting responses of other students on video. The aim of Study 3 was to monitor the influence of cognitive conflict in improving student levels of response. Implications for teachers, educational planners, and researchers are discussed in the light of other researchers' findings.

Introduction

Many research studies over the years have explored people's understanding of random processes. The original studies mainly dealt with college students and their misunderstandings by describing from a psychological perspective how people reason in uncertain conditions, a psychological approach. Later studies were conducted by mathematics educators on data sets composed of school students, some showing little change in understanding across grades. The desire to improve students' statistical understanding and reasoning motivated this research, an educational approach. These two different research perspectives and the contributions they make to the literature on probability and statistics have been considered in some detail by Shaughnessy (1983, 1992). After a review of the place of the random concept in the school curriculum and of previous research on the concept, this study uses familiar tasks to extend previous research in three directions. First, a development model is proposed that displays increasingly complex understandings of the concept of random as involved in the tasks presented. Second, longitudinal interview data are used to monitor the developmental change in understanding over three or four years. Third, cognitive conflict from other students is introduced as a means of testing the tenacity of beliefs and their susceptibility to change. Background for these three avenues of research is also provided.

Random in the School Curriculum

As a key probability concept, random is notoriously difficult for students to grasp. Used as an adjective, more attention is often given to the associated noun, such as in "random sample," to give meaning to a complex idea (Batanero, Green, & Serrano, 1998). The word is often used colloquially in non-mathematical contexts to convey a meaning of haphazard. This is reflected to some extent in the nebulous nature of some dictionary definitions, for example, "Made, done without method or conscious choice" (Waite, 1998, p. 530). Indeed like many concepts that are difficult to define, it seems easier to define random by considering "antonyms to randomness" and exploring what is not random, for example, "order," "organization," and "predictability" (Falk, 1991). Curriculum documents are generally guilty of discussing random events, and random numbers, without specifically defining the term "random" (e.g., Australian Education Council [AEC], 1991; Department for Education, 1995; Ministry of Education, 1992; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 2000). An exception is the Mathematics Guidelines K-8: Overview to Chance and Data of the Department of Education and the Arts [DEA] in the Australian state of Tasmania, which follows closely the model provided by Moore (1990).

The focus on chance in this Strand is to develop in students the ability
to describe randomness and to measure (quantify) uncertainty. Phenomena
or events which may individually have uncertain outcomes (for example,
tossing a coin), but that have a regular pattern of outcomes over the
long term, are referred to as being random. … 

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Development of Reasoning about Random Events
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.