From "Easy" to "Difficult" or Vice Versa: The Case of Infinite Sets

By Tsamir, Pessia | Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, Spring 2003 | Go to article overview

From "Easy" to "Difficult" or Vice Versa: The Case of Infinite Sets


Tsamir, Pessia, Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics


It is commonly agreed that students' ways of thinking should be taken into account when planning instruction and that teachers should choose or construct sequences of instruction for use and discussion in class. The NCTM document, "Principles and Standards for School Mathematics" (2000, p. 17), for instance, specifies that teachers "need to understand the different representations of an idea, the relative strengths and weaknesses of each, and how they are related to one another ... They need to know the ideas with which students often have difficulty and ways to help bridge common misunderstandings". However, the shift from such theoretical knowledge to the design of teaching sequences and to practice is not a trivial one in the least.

For example, one important facet of mathematical knowledge is the ability to move flexibly among different representations of a given mathematical notion, process or problem. Still, it has been widely documented that different representations of the same mathematical problem often trigger different and sometimes even conflicting solutions (e.g., Even, 1998; Janvier, Girardon, & Morand, 1993). Research findings clearly indicate how students tend to respond to the different representations of the same mathematical task and their justifications for these responses (Duval, 1983; Hart, Johnson, Brown, Dickson, & Clarkson, 1989; Tirosh & Tsamir, 1996). A question that arises is how the accumulated data on students' reactions to different representations of given mathematical tasks can be applied to instruction.

1. Using Different Representations in Instruction

Two methods that use the accumulated findings indicating that different representations lead to different results, are teaching by analogy and teaching by cognitive conflict. In both methods, students are presented with different representations of the same task: one is the target task, known to commonly trigger an incorrect response (a difficult task), and the other is the anchoring task--a task known to intuitively trigger a correct response (an easy task). Teachers then attempt to promote students' awareness of the fact that they are being presented with the same task, albeit in different representations.

When going "From Difficult to Easy" in the cognitive conflict teaching approach, students are first asked to solve the target task and then the anchoring task. The students frequently reach two different solutions. Thus, in a subsequent discussion, teachers try to lead them to identify the conflicting elements in their different solutions, and to resolve the conflict according to the relevant mathematical theory. The challenges are to promote students' awareness that the two tasks are essentially the same, and that their responses are incompatible with each other, then to lead them to resolve the conflict in accordance with the formal, mathematical framework (see, for instance, Swan, 1983; Tirosh & Graeber, 1990). It should be noted that, in the literature the terms "anchoring task" and "target task" were defined and used with reference to the From Easy to Difficult teaching (by analogy) approach.

When going "From Easy to Difficult" in the teaching by analogy approach, the students are first asked to solve the anchoring task and then the target task. The teacher preferably chooses or constructs a sequence of bridging tasks that gradually lead from the anchoring task to the target task. Such a sequence of steps may assist the students in reaching the same correct solution to the same task presented in different ways. Indeed, it has been reported that by following such a "From Easy to Difficult" sequence of instruction, students frequently reach correct solutions to all tasks presented in the sequence, including the target task. The challenge is to find suitable anchoring and bridging tasks, and to verify that at each stage students make the mathematical connections that allow them to grasp the "sameness" of the tasks (see, for instance, Clement, 1993; Stavy, 1991). …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

From "Easy" to "Difficult" or Vice Versa: The Case of Infinite Sets
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.