Reality Television and Third-Person Perception

By Leone, Ron; Peek, Wendy Chapman et al. | Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, June 2006 | Go to article overview

Reality Television and Third-Person Perception


Leone, Ron, Peek, Wendy Chapman, Bissell, Kimberly L., Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media


Scores of third-person perception studies have demonstrated the fact that individuals believe other people are more affected by media messages than they themselves are (Perloff, 1993). From Davison's (1983) early work with persuasive media to a variety of studies incorporating material that could be seen as harmful (i.e., pornography, rap music, television violence), researchers continue to explore dimensions of third-person perceptions and effects. Researchers have looked at third-person perception and several types of media messages, including campaign messages (Salwen, 1998), negative political advertising (Cohen & Davis, 1991), product commercials and public service announcements (Gunther & Thorson, 1992), advertising (Brosius & Engel, 1996), a television miniseries (Lasorsa, 1989), rap music (Eveland, Nathanson, Detenber, & McLeod, 1999; McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997), pornography (Gunther, 1995; Lo & Wei, 2002; Rojas, Shah, & Faber, 1996), television violence (Hoffner et al., 1999; Hoffner et al., 2001; Rojas et al., 1996; Scharrer, 2002), and news (Driscoll & Salwen, 1997; Hoffner et al., 1999; Hoffner et al., 2001; Neuwirth & Frederick, 2002; Perloff, 1989).

In their meta-analysis, Paul, Salwen, and Dupagne (2000) categorized media messages used in third-person effect studies into three groups: "socially desirable," "socially undesirable," or "neither socially desirable or undesirable" (p. 67). These three categories are of particular interest here as the current authors consider where to fit reality television into this spectrum, a genre that although enormously popular, is also popularly reviled. Although some critics argue that reality programming is a positive force, "releasing everyday voices into the public sphere" (Dovey, 2000, p. 83), this view is countered by those who point to its stereotyped representations of gender and class, its decontextualization of social problems, and its general degradation of public taste (Dovey, 2000). Yet, it lacks a clear-cut antisocial or immoral dimension like pornography, television violence, or death metal music, so that, unlike these controversial topics, reality programming has elicited no calls for censorship, escaping the scrutiny of crusaders and Congress alike. Reality shows are not meant to inform or educate (like news), not produced in order to persuade or influence (like political advertisements), and do not necessarily or instinctively evoke negative feelings in subjects when used by researchers as a stimulus as media violence, pornography, or death metal lyrics do. Because reality television is generally seen as lowbrow but innocuous, the question of what types of perceptual gaps it produces may point to the difficulty of locating entertainment media in Paul et al.'s "middle" category when conducting third-person research. For reality television viewers, where does it fall on the spectrum of social desirability (Paul et al., 2000), evidenced by the presence, or lack thereof, of a third-person perceptual gap?

Although many studies point out the array of media content that yield third-person effects, rarely is one used anymore that does not fall into one of the categories noted earlier. Reid and Hogg's (2005) recent work addresses similar issues, using self-categorization theory as the basis for three studies involving university students. Similar to the current study, they chose media content and target groups that "matched" normatively. For example, they had students judge the influence of the National Enquirer, Wall Street Journal, and Friends (Study 1) and The Jerry Springer Show, CNBC's Financial News, and MTV (Study 2) on themselves and three groups--other university students, bankers, and "trailer trash." In the two studies, respondents perceived trailer trash to be most influenced by National Enquirer and Jerry Springer, bankers most influenced by the Wall Street Journal and CNBC's Financial News, and other students most influenced by Friends and MTV. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Reality Television and Third-Person Perception
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.