Knowledge Representation and Problem Solution in Expert and Novice Youth Baseball Players

By French, Karen E.; Nevett, Michael E. et al. | Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, December 1996 | Go to article overview

Knowledge Representation and Problem Solution in Expert and Novice Youth Baseball Players


French, Karen E., Nevett, Michael E., Spurgeon, John H., Graham, Kathy C., Rink, Judith E., McPherson, Sue L., Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport


Children enter youth sport with little knowledge of sport tactics. Basic knowledge of the rules, terminology, tactics, and goals of the game are acquired during early participation. Because sport knowledge has been shown to be related to other critical cognitive aspects of game performance (French & Thomas, 1987; McPherson & Thomas, 1989; Starkes & Allard, 1993), much more research is needed to describe and explain how knowledge content and structure are developed.

Verbal protocols of participants' thought during problem solving have been used extensively in cognitive and developmental psychology to examine knowledge content, structure, and complex information processing (Anderson, 1982; Chi, Bassok, Lewis, Reitman, & Glaser, 1989; Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982; Chi, Hutchinson, & Robin, 1989; Voss, 1988). The theoretical framework and underlying assumptions, methods for treatment of data, and terminology used in these paradigms are presented in considerable detail in Ericcson and Simon (1993). More recently, techniques which combine sport problem solving and verbal protocol paradigms have been developed by McPherson (1993a, 1994) These paradigms offer the opportunity to more thoroughly describe the development of sport knowledge.

Game situation interviews are one of the verbal protocol techniques used to examine the content and structure of sport knowledge (McPherson, 1993b, 1994; McPherson & Thomas, 1989). Participants receive an initial set of game conditions. For example, in baseball, game conditions might include the location of runners on base, outs, score, inning, and location where the ball is hit in play. The subject is then asked to generate or recall the most appropriate sequence of game or player actions to solve these game situations. The verbalized thoughts of participants as they recall the solution are used as data.

Successful problem solution would require the subject to select the relevant information in the game conditions, direct attention to activate relevant concepts, and integrate the critical input with associated appropriate action sequences from long-term memory. In less complex game situations, integration might involve matching the relevant game conditions with appropriate actions that are easily and quickly retrievable. Integration in more complex game situations might involve activation of critical input and nodes within long-term memory to form an initial representation of the problem or more abstract classification of problem type (i.e., double play, nonforce situation, preventing a critical run). The problem representation would subsequently guide further search and retrieval operations as relevant information is accessed into and out of working memory, until the sequence of appropriate actions could be verbalized completely.

Several errors can occur during this task. First, individuals can fail to recognize and direct attention to the most relevant game conditions. Second, associations (links) between the relevant game conditions and the most appropriate actions may be weak. Weak associations would lead to poor search and retrieval operations. In this case, the more appropriate game actions may be present in long-term memory, but they are difficult to retrieve. Third, the most appropriate game actions may not be present in long-term memory. Less effective game actions that are present in long-term memory would be accessed instead. Thus, an important variable to consider in responses is what game actions are verbalized, whether the actions are relevant or irrelevant to the game problem. Actions (relevant and irrelevant) that are accessed most frequently in game situations indicate that these actions are in long-term memory, have high activation weights, and are easily retrieved.

These types of errors serve as indicators of cognitive processes involved in problem solution. Although the cognitive processes (i.e., search and retrieval) can not be observed directly, processes may be inferred from the content of verbalized thoughts and the order in which they are verbalized (Ericcson & Simon, 1993).

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Knowledge Representation and Problem Solution in Expert and Novice Youth Baseball Players
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.