Symmetry and Humans: Reply to Mithen's 'Sexy Handaxe Theory.'

By Hodgson, Derek | Antiquity, March 2009 | Go to article overview

Symmetry and Humans: Reply to Mithen's 'Sexy Handaxe Theory.'


Hodgson, Derek, Antiquity


In reply to Machin's criticism of Kohn and Mithen's (1999) 'Sexy Handaxe Theory' in a recent Antiquity debate (Machin 2008: 761-6), Mithen (2008: 766-9) states that sexual selection is still relevant to the symmetry of Acheulean handaxes because this provides the only theory that can account for the various features typical of such artefacts. This conclusion may be misconceived, however, due to the conflation of the various factors relating to symmetry, attractiveness, and health. Crucially, recent studies have not found a genetic link based on sexual selection for physical traits based on symmetry. For example, Koehler et al. (2002) established that there was no difference in preference for the symmetry of male faces by females nearing conception compared to those females taking contraceptives. Similarly, Rhodes et al. (2001) found that, although there might be a link between facial symmetry and perceived health, there was no correlation between facial symmetry and actual health. So, although symmetrical faces may be attractive there have been no studies that support a link between facial symmetry and real health (Valentine et al. 2004; Rhodes 2006: 214). In addition, it has been established that it may not be symmetry but averageness that determines whether a face is regarded as attractive (Rhodes et al. 1999). Interestingly, a number of researchers reject the symmetry hypothesis because perfect symmetrical faces seem to be regarded as less rather than more attractive (Langlois et al. 1994; Swaddle & Cuthill 1995). Furthermore, facial attractiveness and symmetry, instead of signalling health, may have evolved more as a way of disguising health problems in order to manipulate perceivers and therefore may not serve as a reliable means of gauging fitness (Kalick et al. 1998). Crucially, symmetry in males does not prove that preference for symmetry evolved for the purpose of assessing genetic worth; rather, this preference may have arisen as a by-product of selection for the purpose of improved perception and recognition (Enquist & Arak 1994). The purported advantage gained by females in mating with more symmetric males may therefore be a fortuitous effect arising out of a more generalised sensory predilection deriving from a 'perceptual bias' for symmetry (Valentine et al. 2004). Anatomical symmetry may thus have little to do with adaptive fitness or genetic quality. This conclusion is reinforced by the fact that symmetry also does not seem to serve as a signal of health in animals (Polak 2003; Tomkins & Simmons 2003).

Mithen also criticises Machin for failing to suggest an alternative theory that could account for the different factors associated with Acheulean handaxes. Such a theory does in fact exist, based on the notion that symmetry and sexual selection are not necessarily linked. In this regard, Machin's finding that the enhanced symmetry of Acheulean handaxes goes beyond the functional needs of butchering may not imply, as Mithen proposes, that this supports sexual selection; rather, this may be evidence of an interest in symmetry for other, more fundamental, reasons. Reber (2002; Reber et al. 2004), for example, has put forward the concept of perceptual fluency for the fact that the preference for symmetry derives from a tendency predicated on more basic concerns than sexual selection that is consistent with a 'perceptual bias' account of why symmetry is important. From this perspective, symmetry provides a crucial means by which things in the world can be identified in that many biological objects are initially recognised by way of symmetry (Enquist & Arak 1994; Wagemans 1997; Beck et al. 2005). Thus, the ability to detect symmetry has been found to precede conscious awareness in that it is rapid and automatic, to the extent that 4-months old infants are able to discern such shapes (Beck et al. 2005) and brain-damaged patients suffering neglect have an implicit, yet unconscious, awareness of symmetry (Driver et al. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Symmetry and Humans: Reply to Mithen's 'Sexy Handaxe Theory.'
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.