What Makes a Good Story? Supporting Oral Narratives of Young Children from Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Backgrounds

By Cheatham, Gregory A.; Jimenez-Silva, Margarita | Childhood Education, Summer 2011 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

What Makes a Good Story? Supporting Oral Narratives of Young Children from Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Backgrounds


Cheatham, Gregory A., Jimenez-Silva, Margarita, Childhood Education


All children come to school with the language and culture of their homes and communities. However, the formal uses of language in schools often favor children who speak in ways that meet teachers' expectations (Corson, 2001). For example, the story structure used many children can be a mismatch with teachers' expectations, resulting in teacher and child frustration and the inability of some children to demonstrate their linguistic and cognitive skills. To ensure equity, early educators can consider diverse children's narratives from a difference rather than deficit perspective; that is, teachers can view the differences in these narratives as a strength, rather than as an indicator of a child's intrinsic lack of narrative ability. Teachers should seek out ways to support all children's storytelling (Jimenez-Silva, 1996; Jimenez-Silva & McCabe, 1996; McCabe & Bliss, 2003).

In this article, we discuss definitions and characteristics of children's narratives, the importance of young children's narrative skills, and cultural differences as illustrated by two children's example narratives. We demonstrate how knowledge of different types of culture-based story structure and a new pedagogical approach can help early educators support the narratives of young children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds.

Definitions and Characteristics of Narratives

Children can choose from among several types of oral narratives, including event casts (i.e., telling what they are doing as they do it) (Westby, Moore, & Roman, 2002) and fictional narratives (i.e., recall of a story read previously) (McCabe, Bliss, Barra, & Bennett, 2008). Here, we discuss those stories closest to children's experiences--personal narratives, descriptions or retellings of actual events experienced by the child or someone the child knows (Bliss & McCabe, 2008; Labov, 1972; Peterson & McCabe, 1983). In early childhood classrooms, children often can be heard narrating their experiences. During show-and-tell or sharing time, a child may talk about his/her play with a favorite puppet or about a recent family outing. Early educators may support and scaffold these children's stories, prompting them to provide greater detail. During these activities, peers typically listen quietly, although they may ask questions when the story is complete. At home, children's narratives include telling parents about their day at school or recounting for an uncle how they skinned a knee.

These narratives can be thought of as having several dimensions: topic maintenance, event sequencing, informativeness, referencing, conjuctive cohesion, and fluency (McCabe & Bliss, 2003). (See Table 1 for narrative dimensions and definitions.) We have focused on the first four of these dimensions, and on how children's age and development naturally affect their narrative competencies within these dimensions.

Importance of Children's Narratives

As noted by McCabe and Bliss (2003), narratives serve academic functions, particularly related to language and literacy development. Oral narratives are entry points to literacy for young children (Michaels, 1981). Telling a story requires planning, organization, meaning generation, and self-monitoring (Hadley, 1998), which are all skills needed for literacy. Indeed, studies have illustrated that children's narrative skills are positively related to literacy skills (e.g., Tabors, Snow, & Dickinson, 2001).

Additionally, children's oral narratives serve an important role in language socialization (McCabe & Bliss, 2003). At home, narratives are a primary source of interaction and language socialization within families and communities. Hymes (1974) asserted that starting from infancy, children learn culturally specific behaviors, including language use, through interactions with adults and other individuals. Adults' interactions (e.g., adults' oral narrative solicitations and ongoing prompts) serve as the basis for culturally appropriate language acquisition (Minami, 2002) and allow children to communicate, participate, and gain community acceptance (Heath, 1986).

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

What Makes a Good Story? Supporting Oral Narratives of Young Children from Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Backgrounds
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?