Comparative Effectiveness of Phonological Awareness and Oral Language Intervention for Children with Low Emergent Literacy Skills

By Fielding-Barnsley, Ruth; Hay, Ian | Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, October 2012 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

Comparative Effectiveness of Phonological Awareness and Oral Language Intervention for Children with Low Emergent Literacy Skills


Fielding-Barnsley, Ruth, Hay, Ian, Australian Journal of Language and Literacy


Introduction

Early identification of reading difficulties is paramount if teachers are to help change the current situation where 16% of children are falling into the category of 'disabled readers' in Australia. (Louden et al., 2000). In the United States, The National Assessment for Educational Progress (NAEP, 1998) identified 60% of students scoring below proficient in fourth grade. Early identification and the resulting remediation of reading difficulties have many advantages over leaving children to 'grow out' of their problems (Pressley, 2006; Torgesen, 1998). One such advantage is reducing the children's negative feelings associated with their constant failure in the classroom (Hay, 2000; Muter, 2003). Muter maintained that early identification also allowed for a 'purer' child profile to be established, because older children often acquired compensatory and avoidance behaviours that can mask their reading difficulties.

It has been demonstrated that there is a strong link between phonological awareness and early reading. Children's awareness of the phonological units of speech, particularly rhyme (Bradley & Bryant, 1983) and alliteration (Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1995) have been found to have significant effects for early reading. Knowledge of the alphabet is also one of the best predictors for a successful transition into early reading (Adams, 1990).

Whilst there has been a plethora of research on the benefits of phonological awareness intervention for struggling readers (see Lovett, Steinbach & Frijters (2000) for a summary), there has been significantly less on the effects of language intervention (Muter, Hulme, Snowling, & Stevenson, 2004). The NICHD (2005) suggests that there is a vital link between early oral language and decoding skills. While phonological awareness, the understanding of the sound structure of language, is necessary, it alone is not sufficient for the successful acquisition of reading (Byrne, Fielding-Barnsley & Ashley, 2000). Hay, Elias, Fielding-Barnsley, Homel, and Frieberg, (2007) demonstrated that the inclusion of a structured language programme along with a shorter phonological awareness programme produced significant gains in children's reading ability; particularly if those children came from low (socio-economic status) SES homes. Children who fail to respond to reading intervention are often those with weak oral language (Al Otaiba & Fuchs, 2006). Hart and Risley (1995) made the link between weak oral language and low SES in their seminal study where it was found that children from high socio-economic status (SES) families heard around 487 utterances per hour, compared to 178 utterances per hour for children from families on welfare. By the time they were aged 4 years, the high SES children had been exposed to around 44 million utterances, compared to 12 million utterances for the lower SES children.

In particular, the evidence suggests that many children with significant reading and learning difficulties/disabilities have deficits in both phonological awareness and language skills (Saada-Robert, 2004; Snowling, 2005). Whilst language delays are considered a cause of reading delays, the children's lack of reading skills also have an ongoing negative influence on the children's vocabulary and language development (Catts & Kamhi, 2005). This reciprocal relationship between language and reading has significant implications for the type and range of screening and interventions teachers provide to children in the beginning school years.

Several researchers have reported on the beneficial effects of language instruction on measures of comprehension (Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Muter et al., 2004) and more recently Bowyer-Crane et al. (2008) have reported differential effects between an oral language and phonology with reading based intervention. In the Boyer-Crane et al., study the Phonology+Reading group showed an advantage over the Oral Language group on measures of literacy and phonology and the Oral Language group showed an advantage on measures of vocabulary and grammatical skills.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

Comparative Effectiveness of Phonological Awareness and Oral Language Intervention for Children with Low Emergent Literacy Skills
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?