Information-Seeking, Information Literacy and Cognitive Psychology

By Shenton, Andrew K. | School Librarian, Summer 2013 | Go to article overview

Information-Seeking, Information Literacy and Cognitive Psychology


Shenton, Andrew K., School Librarian


Introduction

No doubt many readers frequently find themselves encountering material from other fields that they recognise is of value to their own work in LIS. Only a few months ago, I myself realised how an appreciation of the phenomenon of 'blended memory', which is associated with the discipline of psychology, can help us to understand why some quests for information end in failure. An article in which I explore this territory has now appeared in an education journal. (1) Psychology would seem, in fact, to be an especially rich source of concepts that may be pertinent to our comprehension of information behaviour. Indeed, in a seminal report, Tom Wilson and Christina Walsh single out psychology, along with sociology, as a field that can make a strong contribution in this context. (2)

Any reader who listened to the episode of All in the Mind broadcast on BBC Radio Four on 4 December 2012 may well have been struck by the relevance of the ideas of cognitive scientist Stephan Lewandowsky to the way in which information-seekers deal with inaccurate material. (3) It should be stressed from the outset that the real focus of the discussion on All in the Mind was that of why the brain retains incorrect material, not the evaluation of the content of sources accessed during information-seeking activity, and the programme was thus concerned with processes which are subsequent to those typically of interest to scholars of information behaviour. Nevertheless, several of the issues raised can be recast in terms that are appropriate to school librarians intent on developing their pupils' information literacy.

Lewandowsky's Relevant Principles

Expectations

According to Lewandowsky, the 'default assumption' when receiving information is to accept that the material presented is true. This mindset, he maintains, is partly the result of upbringing--generally, children are raised up by parents who, most of the time, tell them the truth. There is much evidence in the literature associated with LIS to corroborate Lewandowsky's claim in relation to our attitudes to information. Even when children are working with paper sources, David Wray and Maureen Lewis note that they often encounter material that is misleading, incorrect or biased, yet they will still 'naturally tend to believe that everything they read in books written by adults who know a great deal more than them about a particular topic is bound to be true. (4) The lack of trustworthiness of much of the information that can be found on the Web is well known but the pattern which typically emerges in research reports is that when working in this environment, too, youngsters do not adopt a questioning attitude to the material they encounter. Summarising the results of various projects for a major study, Peter Williams and Ian Rowlands highlight the lack of attention that young people give to the authority of a source when using the Internet for information. (5)

[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]

2. Cognitive effort

In some ways, the second principle follows logically from the first. Since the recipient of information is inclined to believe the material presented to them, they must make a special effort to tag the information as false if it becomes obvious that it is inaccurate. It is especially likely, Lewandowsky suggests, that the individual will accept incorrect information when they are distracted or paying insufficient attention. If we acknowledge that the process of recognising that particular information is inaccurate imposes a significant burden on the individual, we may view the person's inclination to ignore the extra demand that is made as consistent with the 'principle of least effort', which is frequently used in LIS literature to explain much information behaviour. Reflecting on the findings of a range of studies, Eliza Dresang suggests that young people have often been found to display 'a propensity to take the easiest path possible'. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Information-Seeking, Information Literacy and Cognitive Psychology
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.