Adolescent Egocentrism and Cognitive Functioning during Late Adolescence

By Rycek, Robert F.; Stuhr, Sherry L. et al. | Adolescence, Winter 1998 | Go to article overview

Adolescent Egocentrism and Cognitive Functioning during Late Adolescence


Rycek, Robert F., Stuhr, Sherry L., McDermott, Jacci, Benker, Jennifer, Swartz, Michelle D., Adolescence


According to Elkind (1967), adolescent egocentrism, which includes a belief by teenagers that they are special and unique, accompanies the attainment of new mental abilities. Specifically, Elkind proposed that adolescents construct an "imaginary audience," giving rise to heightened self-consciousness. Adolescents believe that others, especially peers, are watching them, thinking about them, and interested in all their thoughts and behaviors. Elkind suggested that this is due, in part, to emerging formal operational thought, which allows adolescents to think about their own thinking and that of others. Adolescent egocentrism actually represents a flaw in their thinking that is characteristic of early formal operations. Adolescents assume that since they spend a considerable amount of time thinking about themselves, others must be doing the same thing, namely, thinking about and monitoring them. They fail to realize that while they may be preoccupied with themselves, others are not so inclined.

Previous studies (e.g., Elkind & Bowen, 1979) have generally found that adolescent egocentrism is more prevalent in females and that it increases during early adolescence, peaks at about 14 to 16 years of age, and then decreases during later adolescence. However, a study by Peterson and Roscoe (1991) found egocentrism in female college freshmen to be at a level higher than is typically found in some high school students. This result is somewhat surprising, given the expectation that adolescent egocentrism should have been declining.

A number of studies have examined the relationship between formal operations and adolescent egocentrism, with mixed results. Hudson and Gray (1986) and Riley, Adams, and Nielsen (1984) found significant relationships between formal operations and adolescent egocentrism, while studies by Jahnke and Blanchard-Fields (1993) and Lapsley, Milsread, Quintana, Flannery, and Buss (1986) did not. All of these studies used traditional measures of formal operations to examine the relationship between adolescent egocentrism and cognitive development. This may have been problematic, since adolescents and adults often do poorly on formal operational tasks due to reasons other than their competency.

The present study was designed to examine the relationship between adolescent egocentrism and cognitive level in college students. A post-formal operational assessment (Kramer, Kahlbaugh, & Goldston, 1992) that is not task based, but does discriminate between formistic (early formal to formal operations), mechanistic (formal to late formal operations), relativistic (early post-formal operations), and dialectical (post-formal operations) thinking, was used. It was felt that this measure would be more appropriate for a college-aged population than would traditional tests of formal operations. If there is a relationship between adolescent egocentrism and formal operational thought, then the lower forms of reasoning (formistic and mechanistic) should show greater egocentrism as compared with higher forms of reasoning (relativistic and dialectical).

METHOD

Participants were 163 undergraduate students (53 males and 110 females), ranging in age from 17 to 24 (mean age = 19.6), recruited from introductory psychology classes at a medium-sized midwestern university. They were administered a version of Elkind and Bowen's (1979) Imaginary Audience Scale (IAS) that had been slightly modified for college-aged individuals (similar to the adjustment made by Peterson & Roscoe, 1991). The IAS, which measures adolescent egocentrism, is a 12-item paper-and-pencil self-report instrument. Potentially self-revealing or embarrassing situations are presented, and respondents choose from three possible reactions. The IAS includes two subscales - the Abiding Self (AS), which reflects long-lived characteristics such as mental ability and personality traits, and the Transient Self (TS), which reflects momentary or temporary appearance and behavior such as verbal mistakes and physical mishaps. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Adolescent Egocentrism and Cognitive Functioning during Late Adolescence
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.