The Effects of an Intervention to Advance Moral Reasoning and Efficacy

By Grier, Leslie K.; Firestone, Ira J. | Child Study Journal, December 1998 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

The Effects of an Intervention to Advance Moral Reasoning and Efficacy


Grier, Leslie K., Firestone, Ira J., Child Study Journal


The effects of an Initial test of a dual intervention program designed to enhance moral reasoning and personal efficacy among fifth- and sixth-grade children was examined to determine whether the intervention would advance behavior reflective of moral attributes. Training In moral reasoning utilized a cognitive conflict procedure where children were exposed to higher levels of reasoning through participation in small group discussions. For efficacy training, children were guided in their work on a series of before/after stories, and identified appropriate means to achieve pre-designated goals. The results indicated the treatment group advanced in several of the efficacy measures as well as in behavioral conduct. Several recommendations are provided for future training programs of this nature.

This experiment was an initial test of a dual intervention program designed to advance children's moral reasoning and efficacy. The intervention was based upon Kohlberg and Candee's (1984) model of how moral judgments become transformed into moral action. The process begins with the explication of moral principles which coincides with a moral choice. The last two components include implementation issues of moral (i.e., judgments of responsibility) and nonmoral (i.e., ego-strength factors) dimensions.

Moral reasoning (e.g., Colby & Kohlberg, 1987) reflects justifications for moral actions and is relevant to ethical conduct. Blasi's (1980) review of studies on the relationship between moral reasoning and delinquent behavior found that overall, the moral reasoning development of delinquent individuals was significantly below their nondelinquent counterparts. Across several of the studies, the majority of delinquents (80%) scored at the preconventional level, while matched controls were more likely to score at the conventional level. An inverse relation between moral maturity and antisocial and aggressive behaviors has also been documented in other studies (e.g., Bear, 1989; MacKinnon & Njaa, 1995; Trevethan & Walker, 1989). Also, Hubbs-Tait and Garmon (1995) found that the moral reasoning level of adolescents was inversely related to reported sexual risk taking (e.g., having sexual intercourse without using a condom). Adolescents higher In moral reasoning were less likely to engage in risky sexual behavior. Increased knowledge of acquired immune deficiency syndrome (AIDS) was associated with less sexual risk taking among high moral reasoners, but AIDS knowledge did not predict risk taking among subjects lower in moral reasoning.

McNamee (1978) in an investigation of helping behavior among college students found that moral reasoning stage predicted both the likelihood and quality of help subjects provided to a confederate In distress. Other studies (e.g.. Elmer & Rushton, 1974; Gibbs, Clark, Joseph, Green, Goodrick, & Makowski, 1986; Rubin & Schneider, 1973; Ma, 1989) report similar patterns in children and adolescents; with those higher in moral reasoning being more altruistic. However, Richards, Bear, Stewart, and Norman (1992) suggest that for school related behaviors, this relation is more curvilinear with fewer classroom conduct problems at Stages 1 and 3 opposed to Singe 2.

The relation of moral reasoning to ethical conduct is not direct. Kohlberg (1969) noted that, "ego strength" factors are a potential impediment to moral conduct. A perceived lack of competence regarding intelligence, physical strength or problem solving ability may inhibit striving towards the achievement of moral Ideals. Empirical support for the importance of competence factors is found in the work of Harris, Mussen, and Rutherford (1976) which portrayed intelligence as a stronger predictor of moral conduct than moral reasoning. Also, Kohlberg and Candee (1984) maintained that judgments of responsibility were required before moral judgments become influential determinants of conduct.

Judgments of responsibility pertain to the self.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

The Effects of an Intervention to Advance Moral Reasoning and Efficacy
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?