Effects of Inductive Multimedia Programs in Mediating Word Problem Translation Misconceptions

By Johari, Abbas | Journal of Instructional Psychology, March 2003 | Go to article overview

Effects of Inductive Multimedia Programs in Mediating Word Problem Translation Misconceptions


Johari, Abbas, Journal of Instructional Psychology


This study examined the effects of two inductive multimedia programs, one including the teaching and using of the coordinate graph and its language, on university students' ability conceptualize variables and create equations from word problems. The programs were designed to address the problem of syntactic and semantic translation misconceptions in world problems. For both treatments, posttest scores were significantly higher on both function construction and variable conceptualization than on the pretest. However, students receiving instructions via the inductive multimedia with graph program scored significantly higher on function construction than did those receiving the multimedia only program. This result is consistent with propositions recognizing the conceptual richness of visuals, specifically the coordinate graph, in mathematics education learning. Results suggest using inductive multimedia program treatments that incorporate many instructional strategies including inquiry learning from data, tutorial, schema, and core representational systems. This study also suggests using inductive multimedia programs that include the coordinate graph teaching strategy for the problem of translation, specifically creation of linear function.

**********

In today's work environment, the ability to think critically, to communicate basic mathematical ideas, and to develop problem-solving strategies is essential (Smith, 1994). Furthermore, for students building toward a career, research indicates that a strong relationship exists between mathematical skills and success in college, regardless of major (Waits & Demana, 1988). However, despite the proven short- and long-term value of such skills, student underpreparedness in mathematics is a continuing and growing problem in higher education (Berenson, Best, Stiff, & Wasik, 1990).

In algebraic problem-solving situations, students find algebraic applications difficult. Most students cannot, in fact, translate rational word problems into simple linear functions (Clement, 1982; Lewis & Mayer, 1987; Lochhead & Mestre, 1988; Mayer, 1982; Wollman, 1983). Further, students simply have difficulty with the concept of variables (Leinhardt et al., 1990; Philip, 1992; Usiskin, 1988); variables are an integral part of the concept of function.

Misconceptions are a typical source of errors. Students maintain are incorrect conceptual systems regarding specific concepts in mathematics. These misconceptions may or may not have been intentionally instructed and have been misunderstood repeatedly by learners (Leinhardt et al., 1990). A considerable amount of research has been done in the area of algebraic misconceptions (e.g., Carlson, 1998; Kaur & Boey Peng, 1994), specifically misconceptions during translation tasks from problem situations to equations (Clement, 1982; Kaput & Sims-Knight, 1983; Lewis & Mayer, 1987; Mayer, 1982; Rosnick & Clement, 1980). Generally speaking, misconceptions, evidenced by persistent error patterns, have a psychological base. According to Piagetian learning theory, learners use their existing cognitive structures and construct new knowledge that will be adapted. The problem is that "learner's existing cognitive structures are difficult to change significantly" (Herscovics, 1989, p. 62).

Misconceptions can also be viewed via current cognitive psychology. Mayer (1992) explained the matured (the 1970s and 1980s) cognitive learning theory as a three-part process: "selecting, organizing, and integrating." According to Mayer, integrating is "connecting the organized [new] information to other familiar knowledge structures already in memory" (1984, p. 33). The problem again may be that learners have difficulty opposing their already-existing cognitive structures during the connecting process, especially if their prior knowledge structures make sense to them. The following paragraphs identify two misconceptions that occur consistently during translation from problem situations to equations. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Effects of Inductive Multimedia Programs in Mediating Word Problem Translation Misconceptions
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.