Cutting Down on Cases of Mistaken Identity

By Cook, Yvonne | The Independent (London, England), April 5, 2011 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

Cutting Down on Cases of Mistaken Identity


Cook, Yvonne, The Independent (London, England)


Yvonne Cook examines the methods being used to improve the accuracy of eyewitness evidence

The phrase "I saw it with my own eyes" can sound convincing in a court of law. But eyewitness identification has been responsible for serious miscarriages of justice. The US Innocence Project, which is dedicated to using DNA evidence to overturn wrongful convictions, estimates that mistaken eyewitness identification plays a role in more than 75 per cent of its cases.

Professor Graham Pike, a member of the Forensic Psychology Research Group based at the Open University, is one of a number of psychologists who have been working with police to reduce the number of misidentifications. He says the key to understanding why eyewitnesses can be so unreliable lies in the psychology of human memory.

" Memory is not like a video of an event which can be replayed endlessly and is always perfect. Human memories alter over time, and are very suggestible," he says. "In a classic experiment carried out by the psychologist Elizabeth Loftus, witnesses were asked to recall details of a road accident they had seen on film. She found that when the witnesses were asked 'how fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?' they remembered the cars as travelling faster than when they were asked 'how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?' Simply changing one word in the question influenced the way people remembered the event."

In the UK, research such as this has transformed the way witness evidence is collected. The police are now aware that asking a witness "What colour hat was the man wearing?" is likely to cause the witness to remember the man as wearing a hat, even if he wasn't. Avoiding such leading questions is standard practice in police interviews today.

Under the Police and Criminal Evidence act, eyewitnesses taking part in an identification parade are routinely told that the person being sought for the crime may or may not be present in the parade line-up. Research has shown that this tactic reduces the number of misidentifications. By contrast, asking witnesses "who do you think it is?" suggests that the guilty person must be present in the line- up, and increases the subconscious pressure to pick someone, even if the guilty person is not there.

Psychologists have also demonstrated that people's memories are less reliable when they are under stress. One direct result of this is the rise of the video identity parade. The Forensic Psychology Research Group worked with West Yorkshire Police in the 1990s to pioneer VIPER, which is now used by more than 30 police forces. Instead of a viewing a line-up of people, the witness or victim is invited to pick the perpetrator from a series of video clips of faces. "This is much less stressful for the witness than coming face to face with the offender at a live identity parade," says Professor Pike. VIPER also builds on research which showed that presenting the faces of suspects one by one reduces misidentification. If witnesses are presented with a number of faces all at once, they will be more inclined to pick out "the best match", and they will tend to do so even if it is not the perpetrator.

The work of the Forensic Psychology Research Group was the inspiration for the BBC2 series Eyewitness, funded by the OU, in which crimes were staged in front of participants and then investigated by the Greater Manchester Police.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

Cutting Down on Cases of Mistaken Identity
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?