Linking Dynamical Perceptual Decisions at Different Levels of Description in Motion Pattern Formation: Psychophysics

By Hock, Howard S.; Ploeger, Annemie | Perception and Psychophysics, April 2006 | Go to article overview

Linking Dynamical Perceptual Decisions at Different Levels of Description in Motion Pattern Formation: Psychophysics


Hock, Howard S., Ploeger, Annemie, Perception and Psychophysics


The relationship between local-level motion detection and higher level pattern-forming mechanisms was investigated with the motion quartet, a bistable stimulus for which either horizontal or vertical motion patterns are perceived. Local-level perturbations in luminance contrast affected the stability of the perceived patterns and, thereby, the size of the pattern-level hysteresis obtained by gradually changing the motion quartet's aspect ratio. Briefly eliminating luminance contrast (so nonmotion was perceived during the perturbation) eliminated pattern-level hysteresis, and briefly increasing luminance contrast (so motion was perceived during the perturbation) increased pattern-level hysteresis. Partially reducing luminance contrast resulted in bistability during the perturbation; pattern-level hysteresis was maintained when motion was perceived, and eliminated when nonmotion was perceived. The results were attributed to local motion/nonmotion perceptual decisions in area Vl affecting the magnitude of the activation feeding forward to motion detectors in area MT, where the stability of pattern-level perceptual decisions is determined by activation-dependent, future-shaping interactions that inhibit soon-to-be-stimulated detectors responsive to competing motion directions.

An ongoing issue in our understanding of visual pattern formation concerns the relationship between different levels of description for the pattern. Although the traditional idea is that the relationship is hierarchical, with larger units built from smaller ones (see, e.g., Biederman, 1987; Cutting, 1986), it also has been argued that processing proceeds in the reverse, top-down direction (Hochstein & Ahissar, 2002; Sanocki, 1993). Upstream areas of the brain have larger receptive fields and respond to more abstract, global properties than do downstream areas (Maunsell & Newsome, 1987; Vogels & Orban, 1996), so different levels of description are likely to have their counterparts in different levels of neural processing.

The present research investigates the feedforward flow of activation from the detection of local element motions to the formation of simple global motion patterns. Psychophysical results in this article and computational simulations of these results in the accompanying article (Nichols, Hock, & Schöner, 2006) indicate that (1) pattern-level hysteresis depends on activation-dependent detector interactions and, therefore, on the magnitude of the activation that feeds forward from local to pattern levels, and (2) local-level dynamical decisions that result in motion or nonmotion being perceived (i.e., local Instability) affect pattern-level dynamical decisions by feeding forward different magnitudes of activation for the same stimulus. Dynamical decisions occur when the activation levels of stimulated detectors are attracted to and maintained near stable, fixed-point values (Hock, Schoner, & Giese, 2003), where each fixed point represents the joint activation state of all detectors relevant to the percept. It is assumed that the percept is embodied in those detectors whose activation is stabilized at values above the threshold level required for perception.

Hock, Kogan, and Espinoza (1997) and Hock, Nichols, and Espinoza (2004) have provided psychophysical evidence that detectors responsive to motion-independent spatial information compete with motion detectors in determining whether motion or nonmotion is perceived. They observed spontaneous switching between nonmotion and motion percepts, motion/nonmotion hysteresis, and the independent adaptation of perceived nonmotion and perceived motion. Our account therefore begins in area V1, where in addition to directionally selective motion detectors (Hubel & Wiesel, 1979) there are detectors responsive to motion-independent spatial information (Movshon, Thompson, & Tolhurst, 1978). Detectors in area MT are predominantly motion sensitive (Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983), so V1 is the more likely of the two sites for motion/nonmotion decisions. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Linking Dynamical Perceptual Decisions at Different Levels of Description in Motion Pattern Formation: Psychophysics
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.