Media Schemas, Perceived Effects, and Person Perceptions

By Meirick, Patrick C. | Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, Autumn 2006 | Go to article overview

Media Schemas, Perceived Effects, and Person Perceptions


Meirick, Patrick C., Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly


Media schemas about the power of the media are widely thought to influence perceived media effects and third-person perception, but only one study has shown this, and it did not consider desirable messages. The current research finds focus group evidence for the existence of additional media schemas relevant to estimating effects of pro-social messages, then examines the relationships between media schemas, perceived media effects of desirable and undesirable messages on self and others, and first-, second-, and third-person perceptions. Results indicate that some media schemas can be applied to perceived media effects of self and others, although not exclusively to desirable or undesirable messages. There was no evidence that these schemas are related to first- or third-person perception, but they seem to be better suited to predict the mutual perceived effects of second-person perception.

For almost as long as there have been mass media, there have been widespread conceptions about their power.1 When King Henry VIII took control of printing presses in England in 1529, he demonstrated the same kind of belief in the media's persuasive power that Davison saw in the white officers who withdrew their black troops after the troops had been targeted by Japanese leaflets in World War II.2 It was observations like this that led Davison to posit a "third-person effect": that people tend to believe media messages have greater effects on others than on themselves (the perceptual component), and that they act on this belief (the behavioral component).3

In the context of the third-person effect, Perloff4 has suggested that people hold a lay theory of media, or media schema, resembling the "hypodermic" model: "To the degree that individuals believe that the average person is susceptible to media or that the media are all-powerful, they can logically infer that others are more vulnerable to media than themselves."5 Media schemas also have been used to explain the third-person effect's "target corollary," how people's estimates of a group's exposure to a message predict their estimates of the message's effect on that group.6 In short, if people assume media are powerful, then exposure equals effects.

However, there is to date only one empirical study of media schemas' role in third-person perceptions.7 Moreover, it would seem that media schemas about the power of the media and vulnerability of others to influence could not easily account for first-person perceptions, wherein others are seen as less influenced by desirable media messages than oneself.8 No one has yet investigated media schemas in the context of desirable messages, but Perloff suggested that there may be media schemas specific to socially desirable content that would contain beliefs about the relative weakness of positive messages and the intractability of the audience.9

Third- and first-person perceptions both deal with differences between perceived media effects on others versus on the self. Some recent research has advocated investigating not just differential but also joint perceived effects.10 No research has yet addressed how media schemas might be related to these "second-person perceptions" of mutual influence.

This study sets out to explore the beliefs audiences hold regarding the effects of anti-social and pro-social media content. A series of focus groups was conducted to determine those beliefs and how they were expressed. Focus group data aided the development of reliable quantitative measures of schemas to test their relationships with perceived media effects as well as third-, first-, and second-person perceptions.

Media Schemas and Perceived Media Effects

Social psychology literature suggests people have multiple schemas for understanding and processing information, not all of which will be activated at any given time. Perceiver, situation, and stimulus characteristics affect what schemas are applied.11 In the case of estimating media effects, there are several relevant features: the topic of the media message, the desirability of the message, the receiver, and more. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Media Schemas, Perceived Effects, and Person Perceptions
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.