Predictability of Locomotion: Effects on Updating of Spatial Situation Models during Narrative Comprehension

By Dutke, Stephan; Rinck, Mike | Memory & Cognition, September 2006 | Go to article overview

Predictability of Locomotion: Effects on Updating of Spatial Situation Models during Narrative Comprehension


Dutke, Stephan, Rinck, Mike, Memory & Cognition


We investigated how the updating of spatial situation models during narrative comprehension depends on the interaction of cognitive abilities and text characteristics. Participants with low verbal and visuospatial abilities and participants with high abilities read narratives in which the protagonist's motions in a fictitious building were either highly predictable or very hard to predict. In Experiment 1, high-ability readers updated spatial situation models only if the protagonist's motions were hard to predict, whereas low-ability readers did so only if the motions were highly predictable. In Experiment 2, facilitated integrative spatial processing compensated for the low-ability participants' resource limitations. As a result, both ability groups updated spatial situation models with hard-to-predict protagonist motions. These results highlight the interactions of cognitive abilities and text characteristics in spatial situation model updating.

There is wide agreement in text comprehension research (Zwaan & Radvansky, 1998) that understanding a text involves the construction of representations at different levels of processing (e.g., Gernsbacher, 1990; Graesser, Milels, & Zwaan, 1997; Graesser, Singer, & Trabasso, 1994; W. Kintsch, 1998; van Dijk & W. Kintsch, 1983; Zwaan, Langston, & Graesser, 1995). The representation of the text surface and the prepositional representation of the text's meaning (text base, or text representation) represent features of the text itself, whereas the situation model, or mental model, represents aspects of the situation the text is referring to (Garnham & Oakhill, 1996). Constructing a valid and coherent model of what the text is referring to is assumed to be a core process of understanding a text, in that the situation model integrates text information with the reader's world knowledge. Furthermore, most researchers agree that these constructive processes interact with representations of the pragmatic communicative context, text genre, reader's goals, and text quality (Graesser et al., 1997). Especially the latter factor produced some (at first glance) counterintuitive effects in that under specific circumstances, deficient expository texts led to a better understanding than did less deficient texts. For example, E. Kintsch (1990) reported that more knowledgeable and more skilled readers wrote better summaries of a poorly organized text than of a well-organized one. Presumably, reading a poorly organized text required more active processing for the text information to be integrated with the reader's background knowledge. A well-organized text might obviate this deep integration, however, in favor of a superficial understanding of the text (cf. Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1989; Mannes & W. Kintsch, 1987). Likewise, McNamara, E. Kintsch, Songer, and W. Kintsch (1996) found that high-knowledge eighth-grade readers benefited more from a minimally coherent version of an expository text than from a fully coherent version. McNamara and W. Kintsch (1996), as well as McNamara (2001), replicated these results for adult readers' comprehension of expository texts. They also provided evidence from reading times for the claim that lack of coherence forced high-knowledge readers to engage in compensatory integration of world knowledge, in order to bridge coherence gaps. Depending on the difficulty of the texts, McNamara et al. (1996) and McNamara (2001) found beneficial effects of less coherent texts in tests that tapped either situation model information or text base information.

The present study was an attempt to extend this research in three directions. First, the above-mentioned studies compared "good" expository texts with "bad" ones. Narrative texts, however, are expected to be coherent, and usually authors are motivated to avoid coherence gaps, except when they wish to produce specific effects, such as surprise or astonishment. Therefore, it would be informative to test whether the compensatory effects found by McNamara and her colleagues also occur with narrative texts that differ in ease of processing, although they have the same degree of coherence. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Predictability of Locomotion: Effects on Updating of Spatial Situation Models during Narrative Comprehension
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.