Do You Remember Proposing Marriage to the Pepsi Machine? False Recollections from a Campus Walk

By Seamon, John G.; Philbin, Morgan M. et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2006 | Go to article overview

Do You Remember Proposing Marriage to the Pepsi Machine? False Recollections from a Campus Walk


Seamon, John G., Philbin, Morgan M., Harrison, Liza G., Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


During a campus walk, participants were given familiar or bizarre action statements (e.g., "Check the Pepsi machine for change" vs. "Propose marriage to the Pepsi machine") with instructions either to perform the actions or imagine performing the actions (Group 1) or to watch the experimenter perform the actions or imagine the experimenter performing the actions (Group 2). One day later, some actions were repeated, along with new actions, on a second walk. Two weeks later, the participants took a recognition test for actions presented during the first walk, and they specified whether a recognized action was imagined or performed. Imagining themselves or the experimenter performing familiar or bizarre actions just once led to false recollections of performance for both types of actions. This study extends previous research on imagination inflation by demonstrating that these false performance recollections can occur in a natural, real-life setting following just one imagining.

Are there limits on fictitious actions that we subsequently remember to be real? Considerable memory research, spurred initially by cases of recovered memories of abuse, has indicated that false memory for actions can be reliably demonstrated. For example, research has shown that when a plausible but false childhood event, such as getting lost in a mall, is embedded in a series of true childhood events, and those events are attributed to a trusted family source, adults can subsequently accept that false event as real (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). Still other studies have shown that more unusual fictitious childhood events, such as running around at a wedding and knocking a punchbowl onto the laps of the bride's parents, can also be falsely remembered by adults when the fictitious action is embedded in a set of real childhood events and attributed to the participants' parents (Hyman & Pentland, 1996). Similarly, when childhood portraits are inserted into photographs to portray an event that never took place, such as a hot air balloon ride with a family member, adults can generate false recollections based on the altered photo (Wade, Garry, Read, & Lindsay, 2002). Typically, such false memories occur when a fictitious event is presented in the context of real events from the distant past, the events are attributed to a trusted source, and, to aid in remembering, the participant is given time and instructions to imagine them repeatedly (for a review, see Brainerd & Reyna, 2005).

Other studies have bypassed the potential problem that a false childhood event might have actually occurred in the past by presenting participants with a series of actions to perform or imagine in a laboratory setting, and later testing the participants' recent memory for both the actions and the source of those actions. Goff and Roediger (1998), for example, presented participants with common objects as they heard simple action statements such as "break the toothpick," heard the statements and performed those actions, or heard the statements and imagined performing the actions. One day later, the participants were given some of the old objects and actions along with some new ones, and they were asked to imagine each action one, three, or five times. Two weeks later, the participants were given a list of actions taken from the first and second sessions, together with new actions, and asked to indicate which actions were presented in Session 1 and, if presented, whether those actions were imagined, performed, or only heard. These researchers found that increasing the number of imaginings of a new action in the second session increasingly led participants to misremember that action as having been performed in the first session, thereby demonstrating false recollection by imagination inflation (see also Sharman, Carry, & Beuke, 2004). Subsequently, Thomas and Loftus (2002) and Thomas, Bulevich, and Loftus (2003) extended these false performance results with a similar procedure and showed that not only would participants misremember imagined common actions (e. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Do You Remember Proposing Marriage to the Pepsi Machine? False Recollections from a Campus Walk
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.