Canceling Updating in the Comprehension of Counterfactuals Embedded in Narratives

By de Vega, Manuel; Urrutia, Mabel et al. | Memory & Cognition, September 2007 | Go to article overview

Canceling Updating in the Comprehension of Counterfactuals Embedded in Narratives


de Vega, Manuel, Urrutia, Mabel, Riffo, Bernardo, Memory & Cognition


Participants were given counterfactual sentences-for example, "If Mary had won the lottery she would have bought a Mercedes car"-or factual sentences-for example, "Because Mary won the lottery, she bought a Mercedes car"-embedded in short narratives. Reading times showed that readers were immediately sensitive to the special status of counterfactual information (Experiment 1). In addition, probe-recognition latencies demonstrated that old information was more accessible in counterfactual than in factual stories, and new information was equally accessible in both kinds of stories (Experiment 2). However, after reading additional clauses, new information became less accessible in counterfactual than in factual stories (Experiment 3). These results suggest that counterfactual events are momentarily represented but are later suppressed and the readers' attention goes back to previous events in the story.

We use language to describe facts: namely, what happened, is happening, or could happen. But we are also very skillful at producing and understanding counterfactual sentences such as "If Mary had won the lottery, she would have bought a Mercedes car."1 Counterfactual meaning has an unreal status, because it refers to a past event that did not happen and to the equally unreal consequence of such event. Counterfactuals might seem a pointless activity: Why do we expend our time and mental resources using expressions that do not describe real events or even possible future events? However, counterfactuals are a pervasive aspect of human mental life. The reasoning and social psychology literature provides wide evidence of the psychological functions of counterfactual thinking (see Byrne, 2002, 2005; Roese, 1997, 2005). Counterfactuals allow us to learn from mistakes; for instance, "If I had prepared for the exam I would have gotten an A score" may persuade the speaker to prepare better for the exam next time (Roese, 1994). By contrasting the outcome of an event with its counterfactual alternatives, people can work out causal judgments (Harris, German, & Mills, 1996). Thus the counterfactual "If only John had been so kind as to bring me to the airport, I would have made it to my plane on time" allows the speaker to explain why she has missed her plane, attributing the responsibility to John's lack of kindness. In the same vein, counterfactual simulations of alternatives could be generated to predict the probability of an event (Kahneman & Tversky, 1982). Finally, counterfactuals are emotional amplifiers that may result in social emotions of regret, guilt, or blame, as well as in the more positive emotions of relief or satisfaction (Byrne, 2002; Guttentag & Ferrell, 2004; Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 2002; Kahneman & Miller, 1986).

In contrast to the amount of attention paid to counterfactual thinking, the study of counterfactual comprehension has been neglected by psycholinguists and discourse researchers. This neglect is not justifiable, because counterfactuals are produced not only to accomplish speakers' personal goals but also to be understood by addressees. The aim of this article is to explore the comprehension of counterfactuals embedded in texts using reading times and accessibility measures. In spite of the lack of specific research on the comprehension of counterfactuals, we will review literature relevant to the present subject. First, we will describe theoretical approaches to counterfactual meaning according to cognitive linguistics and the conditional reasoning literature; second, we will focus on the notion of updating, which is central to explaining the hypotheses of the present research; and finally we will describe studies on the comprehension of mentalist expressions that share characteristics with counterfactuals.

A popular hypothesis in cognitive linguistics is that understanding counterfactuals involves the representation of two possible states of the world (Fauconnier, 1994; Langacker, 1991).

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Canceling Updating in the Comprehension of Counterfactuals Embedded in Narratives
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.