Taking a Closer Look at Vocabulary Learning Strategies: A Case Study of a Chinese Foreign Language Class

By Winke, Paula M.; Abbuhl, Rebekha | Foreign Language Annals, Winter 2007 | Go to article overview

Taking a Closer Look at Vocabulary Learning Strategies: A Case Study of a Chinese Foreign Language Class


Winke, Paula M., Abbuhl, Rebekha, Foreign Language Annals


Abstract:

Over the past decades, there have been a number of studies investigating the vocabulary strategies used by learners of Indo-European languages, especially English. However, studies of the strategies used by learners of non-Indo-European languages are rare. This classroom-based case study investigates the vocabulary learning strategies used by nine learners of Chinese as a foreign language and tests a taxonomy, based on Long's (1996) interaction hypothesis, for classifying strategies that may facilitate our understanding of strategies and their role within second language acquisition.

Key words: Chinese as a foreign language, learning strategies, second language acquisition, vocabulary acquisition

Language: Chinese

Introduction

Given the importance of learning lexical items to second language (L2) acquisition, a considerable body of literature has emerged on various types of vocabulary learning strategies (e.g., Atay & Ozbulgan, 2007; Avila & Sadoski, 1996; Clarke & Nation, 1980; Fan, 2000; Fowle, 2002; Kelly, 1990; Mondria & Mondria-De Vries, 1994; Shapiro & Waters, 2005; van Hell & Mahn, 1997) and on the strategies used by various groups of learners, such as Chinese students learning English (Fan, 2003; Gu & Johnson, 1996), Japanese students learning English (Schmitt, 1997), Canadian students learning French (Harley & Hart, 2000), Spanish-speaking students learning English (Jimenez Catalan, 2003; Rodriguez & Sadoski, 2000), and Australian students learning Italian (Lawson & Hogben, 1996).

A number of these studies have drawn upon Oxford's (1990) commonly used taxonomy of language learning strategies. Oxford divided the domain of language learning strategies (which she defined as conscious "operations employed by the learner to aid the acquisition, storage, retrieval, and use of information . . . ; specific actions taken by the learners to make learning easier, faster, more enjoyable, more self-directed, more effective, and more transferable to a new situation" [p. 8]) into six categories. The first, metacognitive, involves reflecting on the learning process (such as planning and self-evaluation). These are to be distinguished from cognitive strategies, which involve manipulating or transforming learning materials-such as note taking, analysis, and translation. Memory strategies are those used to commit that material to memory (e.g., flash cards). Social strategies involve interacting with another individual, while compensation strategies are methods for overcoming limitations in the L2 (such as using gestures when interacting with a speaker of the target language). Finally, affective strategies entail monitoring "your emotional temperature" (Oxford, 1990, p. 17).

While heavily influenced by Oxford's work, some vocabulary strategy researchers have demarcated the language strategy domain in a slightly different manner. Schmitt (1997), for example, classified strategies as either discovery strategies (strategies used to determine the meaning of a word) or consolidation strategies (strategies used to keep the meaning of the word in memory). Using this taxonomy, Schmitt found that using a bilingual dictionary, guessing from context, and asking classmates for help were the most common discovery strategies, while verbal repetition, written repetition, and studying the spelling of the word were the most frequent consolidation strategies. This bipartite taxonomy also was employed by Harley and Hart (2000), who found that the discovery strategies preferred by their French as a second language learners were social strategies (e.g., asking a friend for the meaning of a word) and inferring (e.g., guessing the meaning of a word from context). With respect to consolidation strategies, the majority of the learners in the study reported that rote learning of English-French word pairs was useful, while repeating words aloud was less helpful. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Taking a Closer Look at Vocabulary Learning Strategies: A Case Study of a Chinese Foreign Language Class
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.