Change Perception in Complex Auditory Scenes

By Pavani, Francesco; Turatto, Massimo | Perception and Psychophysics, May 2008 | Go to article overview

Change Perception in Complex Auditory Scenes


Pavani, Francesco, Turatto, Massimo, Perception and Psychophysics


In four experiments, we examined the role of auditory transients and auditory short-term memory in perceiving changes in a complex auditory scene comprising multiple auditory objects. Participants were presented pairs of complex auditory scenes that were composed of a maximum of four animal calls delivered in free field; participants were instructed to decide whether the two scenes were the same or different (Experiments 1, 2, and 4). Changes to the second scene consisted of either the addition or the deletion of one animal call. Contrary to intuitive predictions based on results from the visual change blindness literature, substantial deafness to the change emerged without regard to whether the scenes were separated by 500 msec of masking white noise or by 500 msec of silence (Experiment 1). In fact, change deafness was not even modulated by having the two scenes presented contiguously (i.e., 0-msec interval) or separated by 500 msec of silence (Experiments 2 and 4). This result suggests that change-related auditory transients played little or no role in change detection in complex auditory scenes. Instead, the main determinant of auditory change perception (and auditory change deafness) appears to have been the capacity of auditory short-term memory (Experiments 3 and 4). Taken together, these findings indicate that the intuitive parallels between visual and auditory change perception should be reconsidered.

Change perception in the visual modality-and particularly its counterpart, change blindness-have received much interest in the past decade (Rensink, 2002; Simons & Levin, 1997). In particular, change blindness studies have shown that, contrary to our impression of seeing everything around us, human observers often fail to see even large changes in the scene if attention is not directed to the object or location of change (O'Regan, Rensink, & Clark, 1999; Rensink, O'Regan, & Clark, 1997). Change blindness does not directly address the conscious perception of objects per se, but addresses only the awareness of change (Rensink, 2000); yet it supports the view that attention might be crucial for the contents of our conscious visual perception, or, put differently, that we consciously see what we attend to (Mack & Rock, 1998). In the present study, we examined an auditory analogue of change blindness-namely, change deafness-in the context of complex auditory scenes.

In the visual modality, the mechanisms allowing correct change perception and those leading to change blindness are relatively well understood (O'Regan, Deubel, Clark, & Rensink, 2000). Any sudden change in the environment that can be registered by the visual system gives rise to a local visual transient. When such a local transient is detected, attention is automatically attracted to the change location, and the novel aspect of the visual scene is reported (Rensink et al., 1997; Turatto & Bridgeman, 2005). However, when the local visual transient is masked or made ineffective in capturing visual attention, the change location remains unattended, and change blindness occurs. Although different visual events can mask the change-related transient (Rensink, 2002), this has typically been achieved by interposing a blank screen between two consecutive images of the same scene differing in only one particular (Rensink et al., 1997). The appearance of the second scene after the blank creates a global visual transient (i.e., all locations in the second scene change with respect to the previous blank image), so that detection of the local visual transient that is associated with the changed object is no longer available to summon attention. Under these conditions, change blindness is often observed, and the change can be detected only through a time-consuming serial scanning of the scenes. According to this account, selective attention is the mechanism that permits information from the two scenes to be stored in visual short-term memory (vSTM; see Luck & Vogel, 1997; Phillips, 1974) for comparisons across views that would then allow conscious change detection. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Change Perception in Complex Auditory Scenes
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.