Putting Probability Back into Probable Cause

By Minzner, Max | Texas Law Review, April 2009 | Go to article overview
Save to active project

Putting Probability Back into Probable Cause


Minzner, Max, Texas Law Review


When deciding whether a baseball player is likely to get a hit, we look at his history of success at the plate. When deciding whether to listen to the advice of a stock analyst, we look at whether the prices of her past recommendations rose or fell. But when police officers claim that they have probable cause to believe a certain location contains evidence of a crime, we do not look at whether they have been right or wrong when they have made the same claim in the past. This is a mistake.

Law-enforcement-search success rates vary widely, even when the same legal standard applies. Searches pursuant to warrants issued on a probable-cause standard recover evidence at very high rates, usually exceeding 80%. By contrast, warrantless searches, even when officers allege they have probable cause, succeed at far lower rates, recovering evidence as infrequently as 12% of the time. Similarly, some officers are far more successful than others when they conduct probable-cause searches. Some almost never recover evidence; some almost always do.

What role should these different success rates play in probable-cause analysis? The current answer is none. Judges are not presented with the success rates of the law enforcement officers who appear before them. I argue that law enforcement should be forced to present success-rate data to judges when making probable-cause claims and judges should be allowed to consider the data when deciding whether to issue a warrant or approve a previously conducted search. These success rates capture information not currently analyzed in the search process and their addition would improve the accuracy of probable-cause decisions. Most significantly, we would learn private information in the possession of law enforcement that is not currently presented to judges.

I. Introduction

You are a magistrate judge. Officer A is seeking a search warrant for a house.1 He knows the house belongs to an individual with a history of narcotics trafficking. When the officer went to the home and interviewed the owner, the owner seemed nervous and appeared to be concealing something behind his back. Officer A tells you that, in his view, this information constitutes probable cause to believe that the house contains narcotics.

Officer B performed a search of an automobile as part of a routine traffic stop. When he pulled the car over, the officer ran the license plates and learned that the car's owner was a known drug dealer. When he approached the car, the driver seemed nervous and appeared to be concealing something beneath his seat. Officer B searched the car and found narcotics.2 He claims the evidence should be admissible because he had probable cause to believe there were narcotics in the vehicle.

These cases may appear similar and may seem close to the probablecause threshold but are, in fact, very different. There is far more reason to rely on Officer A's judgment. When he believes he has probable cause, he is usually right - he recovers evidence almost 90% of the time.3 On the other hand, Officer B is almost always wrong - he recovers evidence just under 12% of the time.4 Searches pursuant to a warrant are far more likely to recover evidence than warrantless searches.

Such variation does not simply exist between different types of searches. Different officers conducting the same type of search succeed at very different rates. Here we can take as an example two real officers of the Florida State Police.5 Between January 2000 and September 2001, each performed a similar number of probable-cause searches of automobiles: Officer A conducted eighteen searches, while Officer B conducted fifteen searches. When Officer A thought he was likely to recover evidence, he was almost always wrong. Only one of his eighteen searches led to a seizure - a success rate of 5.6%. Officer B, by contrast, was almost always right. He recovered evidence in thirteen of the fifteen searches, for an 86.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
Loading One moment ...
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited article

Putting Probability Back into Probable Cause
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

While we understand printed pages are helpful to our users, this limitation is necessary to help protect our publishers' copyrighted material and prevent its unlawful distribution. We are sorry for any inconvenience.
Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.

Are you sure you want to delete this highlight?