Intrinsic Orientation and Study Viewpoint in Recognizing Spatial Structure of a Shape

By Li, Xiaoou; Mou, Weimin et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, June 2009 | Go to article overview

Intrinsic Orientation and Study Viewpoint in Recognizing Spatial Structure of a Shape


Li, Xiaoou, Mou, Weimin, McNamara, Timothy P., Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Two experiments dissociated the roles of intrinsic orientation of a shape and participants' study viewpoint in shape recognition. In Experiment 1, participants learned shapes with a rectangular background that was oriented differently from their viewpoint, and then recognized target shapes, which were created by splitting study shapes along different intrinsic axes, at different views. Results showed that recognition was quicker when the study shapes were split along the axis parallel to the orientation of the rectangular background than when they were split along the axis parallel to participants' viewpoint. In Experiment 2, participants learned shapes without the rectangular background. The results showed that recognition was quicker when the study shape was split along the axis parallel to participants' viewpoint. In both experiments, recognition was quicker at the study view than at a novel view. An intrinsic model of object representation and recognition was proposed to explain these findings.

People experience an object visually from a single viewpoint at any given time. In most situations, the viewpoint from which an object is first experienced is different from the viewpoint from which it is later recognized. Recognition must therefore rely on a representation and a process that can accommodate the change of the viewpoints from study to recognition. The models proposed to conceptualize the nature of such mental representation and process can be divided into two categories: structural description models and view-based models (Hayward, 2003).

Structural description models claim that an object is represented as a set of parts with a specific structural description of the spatial relations among the parts, and recognition relies on matching the structure descriptions between the test object and the objects in memory (e.g., Biederman, 1987; Marr & Nishihara, 1978). For example, a hand can be represented as five fingers and one palm, with specific spatial relations among the parts. The view-based models claim that snapshots of an object are represented at all learning viewpoints and that recognition relies on the normalization between the recognition view and the closest snapshot in memory (e.g., Tarr & Pinker, 1989, 1990; Ullman, 1989).

The view-based models have found support in findings that object recognition performance is better at a familiar view than at a novel view, and that performance at a novel view decreases as the angular distance between the novel view and the closest study view increases (e.g., Cooper, 1975; Jolicoeur, 1988; Tarr & Pinker, 1989, 1990). In contrast, the structural description models have found support in findings that object recognition can be viewpoint independent when it relies on distinctive features that are also viewpoint independent (e.g., the number of parts; see Biederman & Gerhardstein, 1993). Recent studies have shown that object recognition can rely on both structure and view information (e.g., Foster & Gilson, 2002).

In this project, we proposed and tested an intrinsic model of object representation and recognition derived from the intrinsic model of spatial memory (Mou, Fan, McNamara, & Owen, 2008; see also Mou & McNamara, 2002; Mou, McNamara, Valiquette, & Rump, 2004), which claims that interobject spatial relations in a layout of objects are represented with respect to the intrinsic orientation1 (intrinsic reference direction) of the layout. The study location of the observer is also represented with respect to the intrinsic orientation of the layout. Recognizing a previously learned configuration requires the observer to (1) identify the intrinsic orientation of the test scene, (2) align the intrinsic orientation of the test scene with the represented intrinsic orientation of the layout, and (3) compare the spatial relations in the test scene with the represented spatial relations of the layout.

Mou et al. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Intrinsic Orientation and Study Viewpoint in Recognizing Spatial Structure of a Shape
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.