Is a Bear White in the Woods? Parallel Representation of Implied Object Color during Language Comprehension

By Connell, Louise; Lynott, Dermot | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, June 2009 | Go to article overview

Is a Bear White in the Woods? Parallel Representation of Implied Object Color during Language Comprehension


Connell, Louise, Lynott, Dermot, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Color is undeniably important to object representations, but so too is the ability of context to alter the color of an object. The present study examined how implied perceptual information about typical and atypical colors is represented during language comprehension. Participants read sentences that implied a (typical or atypical) color for a target object and then performed a modified Stroop task in which they named the ink color of the target word (typical, atypical, or unrelated). Results showed that color naming was facilitated both when ink color was typical for that object (e.g., bear in brown ink) and when it matched the color implied by the previous sentence (e.g., bear in white ink following Joe was excited to see a bear at the North Pole). These findings suggest that unusual contexts cause people to represent in parallel both typical and scenario-specific perceptual information, and these types of information are discussed in relation to the specialization of perceptual simulations.

Color is an important part of our conceptual representation of objects. Knowledge about color typicality allows us to recognize objects with highly diagnostic colors (e.g., banana or fire engine) more rapidly than objects with no particular diagnostic color (e.g., dog or lamp: Tanaka & Presnell, 1999). Indeed, our conceptual knowledge of an object's typical color is more influential in object recognition than is the color actually perceived (Mapelli & Behrmann, 1997; Tanaka & Presnell, 1999). For example, when participants are primed with a picture of a purple apple (i.e., displayed in an atypical color), they are faster to recognize the word cherry (which shares the prime's typical color red) than they are the word blueberry (which shares the prime's displayed color purple: Joseph & Proffitt, 1996).

However, the presence of context can easily alter conceptual considerations of an object's color. For example, Medin and Shoben (1988) found that people, when asked to compare the color gray with black and with white, considered gray to be more similar to white in the context of hair, but more similar to black in the context of clouds. Similarly, Halff, Ortony, and Anderson (1976) found that people represented the color red differently for hair, wine, flag, brick, and blood, considering the color of a red flag to be more similar to a red light than to a red wine. Such context effects are not limited to simple noun-color combinations, but have also been found for larger scenarios. Research in embodied cognition has shown that people represent implied perceptual information during sentence comprehension even though doing so does not facilitate task performance (Connell, 2007; Stanfield & Zwaan, 2001; Zwaan, Stanfield, & Yaxley, 2002). In the case of color, Connell has shown that short-term representations of object color can affect people's ability to recognize objects. For example, when presented with a sentence that implied a particular color for an object (e.g., Joanne always took milk in her coffee), followed by a picture (i.e., a cup of coffee), people's speed in verifying that the object had been previously mentioned depended on whether the coffee was shown as milky brown or as straight black.

So what happens if our contextual representation of an object conflicts with our canonical knowledge about its typical state? Theories of embodied (grounded) cognition usually describe color representation as the specialization of a perceptual simulation to include color information (Barsalou, 1999, 2008; Zwaan, 2004). That is, the same neural subsystems that represent color in perception are activated to represent color detail in the conceptualization of an object; specifically, fMRI has shown the same region in the left fusiform gyrus to be implicated in both perceptual and conceptual processing of color (Simmons et al., 2007). However, there has been little discussion of how such specialization might take place if the object simulation is already, by default, specialized with a typical color. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Is a Bear White in the Woods? Parallel Representation of Implied Object Color during Language Comprehension
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.