Does Systematic Reading Instruction Impede Prediction of Reading a Shallow Orthography?

By Schabmann, Alfred; Schmidt, Babara Maria et al. | Psychology Science, July 1, 2009 | Go to article overview

Does Systematic Reading Instruction Impede Prediction of Reading a Shallow Orthography?


Schabmann, Alfred, Schmidt, Babara Maria, Klicpera, Christian, Gasteiger-Klicpera, Barbara, Klingebiel, Kathrin, Psychology Science


Abstract

Different methods of reading instructions have been the subject of controversy. This study examined the influence of systematic phonics vs. non-systematic phonics methods of instruction on the prediction of reading. 443 kindergarten children were tested on phonological awareness, naming speed and visual word matching using the Bielefelder Screening (BISC). Children were retested in grades one and four. Results showed that although the prognostic validity of kindergarten measures was generally low, it was considerably higher for grade one children in classes with non-systematic phonics instruction. Children who received systematic phonics instruction scored significantly higher on measures of phonological decoding as compared to their peers who received less systematic instruction. Implications for the prediction of reading and early screenings are discussed.

Key words: reading instruction; prediction of reading; phonics instruction; phonological awareness; rapid automatized naming

Introduction

Early identification of children at risk for reading difficulties is of particular importance. It allows timely intervention to avoid serious reading problems which could otherwise cause failure during early schooling and later academic career (e.g. Schabmann & Kabicher, 2007; Torgesen & Burgess, 1998).

Previous research in English speaking countries has demonstrated that reading achievement can be predicted by early measures of phonological awareness (PA) and naming speed. PA implies the ability to manipulate large (e.g. syllables) and small (e.g. phonemes) units of spoken language, which helps children to understand the grapheme-phoneme coding principle in written language (e.g. Bradley & Bryant, 1985; Bryant, Bradley, MacLean & Crossland, 1989). Early difficulties in PA hence interfere with the acquisition of alphabetic coding, particularly at the beginning of learning how to read. PA tasks include rhyming, syllable counting, phoneme deletion, phoneme blending, and phoneme detection (for further tasks see Landed & Wimmer, 2000; 2008).

To measure naming speed subjects are asked to recall and name letters, digits, objects, or colours as quickly as possible (also called rapid automatized naming, RAN). So far, no consensus about the nature of relationship between RAN and reading has been found. Some authors argue RAN should be considered part of a more general "phonological processing" construct that reflects the access to phonological information in the long term memory (e.g. Torgesen, Wagner & Rashotte, 1998). However, previous studies showed that RAN accounts for additional variance in reading even when controlling for phonological awareness (Bishop, 2003; Blachman, 1984; Bowers, 1995; Bowers & Swanson, 1991; Compton, DeFries & Olson, 2001; Cornwall, 1992; Landed & Wimmer, 2008; Manis, Doi & Badha, 2000; McBride-Chang & Manis, 1996; Wimmer, 1993; Wolf & Bowers, 1999).

Some aumors consider RAN as an indicator of a mechanism mat is essential for automatic processing of orthographic (rather than phonological) representations in memory, i.e. slow letter identification would make it difficult for children to acquire the sensitivity for frequently occurring orthographic patterns (e.g. Bowers & Wolf, 1993; Wolf, Bowers & Biddle, 2000; for a more detailed discussion see Kirby, Parilla & Pfeiffer, 2003 or Georgiou, Parilla, Kirby & Stephenson, 2008; for a recent finding challenging this view see Moll, Fussenegger, Willburger & Landerl, 2009). Other aumors (e.g. Spring & Davis, 1988) assume that RAN might reflect the ability to coordinate multiple concurrent processes (i.e. visual, linguistic and articulatory) or general cognitive processing speed (e.g. Kail, Hall & Caskey, 1999).

For the predictive strength of early PA and RAN measures at least three methodological aspects are critical (Verhagen, Aamoutse & van Leeuwe, 2008): The manner in which PA and RAN are measured, the implemented reading measures (i. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Does Systematic Reading Instruction Impede Prediction of Reading a Shallow Orthography?
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.