Visual Evidence

By Feigenson, Neal | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, April 2010 | Go to article overview

Visual Evidence


Feigenson, Neal, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Visual and other demonstrative evidence has become increasingly prevalent in American and other courtrooms in recent years. However, there have been relatively few experimental studies of the effects of this kind of evidence in legal settings. As a consequence, little is known about when and how it affects legal decision making. In this article, I survey the extant research, including studies of photographs, videos, computer animations, and PowerPoint displays. The research shows that visual evidence affects legal decisions in some circumstances but not in others. It also indicates that visual evidence sometimes enhances legal judgment by improving recall and understanding but sometimes impairs judgment by prompting undue emotional responses, cognitive and perceptual biases, and/or peripheral processing. The limitations of the research are discussed, and directions for future research are suggested.

Visual and other demonstrative evidence, including photographs, drawings, maps, and models, has long been used in court to illustrate the oral testimony of eyewitnesses and experts (Mnookin, 1998).1 It has become increasingly prevalent in recent years, in large part because digital technology has made it easier to record or, in the case of computer animations, to recreate legally relevant reality visually (Feigenson & Spiesel, 2009). A dashboard camera video, for instance, provided the dispositive evidence in a recent Supreme Court decision on the police's authority to use deadly force to terminate a car chase (Scott v. Harris, 2007),2 and a cell phone video will be critical should a lawsuit brought against a New York City police officer for assaulting a bicyclist proceed to trial (Weichselbaum, 2009).3

Although there have been efforts to document courtroom technology generally (e.g., Center for Legal and Court Technology, 2007) and to instruct judges and lawyers in its use (e.g., Federal Judicial Center/National Institute for Trial Advocacy, 2001), there have been relatively few experimental studies of the effects of visual and multimedia evidence in legal settings. As a consequence, little is known about when and how this kind of evidence affects legal decision making. A better understanding should be not only of intrinsic interest to cognitive psychologists but also extremely useful to the legal system, helping policy makers and judges to regulate the use of such evidence more wisely and informing advocates as they decide when and how to present it.

VARIETIES OF VISUAL EVIDENCE

There are good reasons to suspect that visual evidence may influence ultimate judgments of guilt or liability and damages. The police officers who beat Rodney King might never have been brought to trial were it not for George Holliday's videotape of the incident, and the jurors' verdict arguably turned on their interpretations of the tape. As surveillance videos made by both governments and private citizens proliferate, such evidence is likely to make a difference in more and more cases (see Feigenson & Spiesel, 2009). Even where it is not necessarily the focal point of the case, visual evidence would likely influence decision makers' thoughts and feelings and, hence, their verdicts. Because still or moving pictures can be more vivid than oral testimony or text, for instance, visual evidence could well be more memorable and/or lead to stronger emotional responses (see Bell & Loftus, 1985), either of which could affect the outcome. Personal injury lawyers often present day-in-the-life movies of an accident or malpractice victim (e.g., Joseph, 1997), presumably because they believe that the movies give jurors a better understanding of the extent of the victim's injuries and/ or encourage them to sympathize with the victim, leading to larger damage awards. In short, general cognitive psychological research, the practices of experienced trial lawyers, and perhaps common sense all suggest that visual evidence may often affect legal outcomes. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Visual Evidence
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.