How High Is Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity for Object Layout?

By Sanocki, Thomas; Sellers, Eric et al. | Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, May 2010 | Go to article overview

How High Is Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity for Object Layout?


Sanocki, Thomas, Sellers, Eric, Mittelstadt, Jeff, Sulman, Noah, Attention, Perception and Psychophysics


Previous research measuring visual short-term memory (VSTM) suggests that the capacity for representing the layout of objects is fairly high. In four experiments, we further explored the capacity of VSTM for layout of objects, using the change detection method. In Experiment 1, participants retained most of the elements in displays of 4 to 8 elements. In Experiments 2 and 3, with up to 20 elements, participants retained many of them, reaching a capacity of 13.4 stimulus elements. In Experiment 4, participants retained much of a complex naturalistic scene. In most cases, increasing display size caused only modest reductions in performance, consistent with the idea of configural, variable-resolution grouping. The results indicate that participants can retain a substantial amount of scene layout information (objects and locations) in short-term memory. We propose that this is a case of remote visual understanding, where observers' ability to integrate information from a scene is paramount.

The relation between the mind and the world is of central interest in psychology, and scene perception is a multifaceted example of this relation. A critical characteristic of scene perception is the amount of information that can be extracted from a scene and retained in immediate memory. Currently, considerable debate centers on this quantity.

At one end of the debate is the hypothesis that very little is held in memory beyond an attended object (see, e.g., O'Regan, 1992; Wolfe, Klempen, & Dahlen, 2000). In this hypothesis, the intuitive experience of perceiving a rich world is explained by the richness of the information available from the world via eye movements (see O'Regan, 1992). The hypothesis of minimal representation receives support from difficulties of change detection documented in many studies (e.g., Grimes, 1996; Simons, 1996).

At the other end of the debate is the hypothesis that rich representations of scenes result from combined contributions of short-term and long-term memory (e.g., Hollingworth, 2004, 2005, 2007; Irwin & Zelinsky, 2002; Melcher, 2006). Long-term memory contributions can be obviated by experimental design, limiting the represented information to short-term memory. Visual short-term memory (VSTM) for objects has been studied carefully, and many researchers have concluded that it has a sharp limit, between two and four objects (e.g., Hollingworth, 2006; Vogel, Woodman, & Luck, 2001). However, almost all of the studies supporting a sharp limit in VSTM capacity have measured memory for properties of objects. Object properties constitute only one aspect of scene memory. A second fundamental aspect is memory for the layout of objects and other structures in a scene. The representation of spatial information is a fundamental component of short-term memory models (e.g., Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Logie, 1995).

A number of studies suggest that fairly complex layouts of objects can be retained over short-term intervals. In seminal studies of very short-term visual memory, Phillips (1974) also included longer short-term intervals. At the short-term intervals, he found accurate detection of changes of the position of a single element within 25-element arrays (e.g., above 85% in several conditions). Simons (1996) contrasted VSTM for object properties and locations and found location memory, but not object property memory, to be near the ceiling (see also Aginksy & Tarr, 2000; Alvarez & Oliva, 2007; Jiang, Olson, & Chun, 2000). Franconeri, Alvarez, and Enns (2007) recently concluded that as many as seven locations can be held in short-term memory. Rensink (2000b) obtained capacity estimates of at least nine for contrast signs of objects. Brockmole, Wang, and Irwin (2002) presented dot layouts for an integration task and estimated that the number of dots held in VSTM was about 10. A separate line of evidence for complex representations of layout is spatial priming with scenes; Sanocki and colleagues (Sanocki, 2003; Sanocki, Michelet, Sellers, & Reynolds, 2006; Sanocki & Sulman, 2009) found evidence that broadscale representations of layout information are activated in memory by a prime scene. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

How High Is Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity for Object Layout?
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.