An Experimental Investigation of News Source and the Hostile Media Effect

By Arpan, Laura M.; Raney, Arthur A. | Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, Summer 2003 | Go to article overview

An Experimental Investigation of News Source and the Hostile Media Effect


Arpan, Laura M., Raney, Arthur A., Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly


This study examined the interaction among different news sources, individual levels of partisanship, and the hostile media effect in sports news. Two hundred and three participants read a balanced story about their home-town college football team in one of three newspapers: the home-town, the cross-state rival university's town, or a neutral-town paper. The study found differences in the hostile media effect across conditions, suggesting the importance of news source in the phenomenon. Further, findings indicate strong support for the hostile media effect among sports news consumers.

Audience perception of credibility is considered vital to success of a given news outlet. News consumers who consider news outlets more rather than less credible tend to rely on those media, and such reliance can lead to greater media exposure.1 However, American audience evaluations of reporter and news-outlet credibility have generally been less than favorable over the past few decades. A recent poll found that, although general perceptions of media credibility had improved during the past decade, almost half of those surveyed thought the American news media usually report inaccurately, while just over half said they thought members of the press were "moral."2

The American Society of Newspaper Editors responded to rapidly eroding public trust in the news media in 1982 with a study that began to address and define credibility as it pertained to perceptions of media outlets, particularly newspapers.3 More recent research shows, however, that a news outlet's ability to be perceived as credible may stem less from reporting efforts and more from issue involvement and cognitive processes of audience members.4 Specifically, news stories intended by reporters and considered by uninvolved parties to be neutral can be perceived as biased, and therefore lacking in credibility, by highly involved news consumers. The current study used the experimental method to integrate both perspectives-source characteristics and audience involvement-to further explain the hostile media effect.

Literature Review

The process by which some news consumers rate ostensibly neutral stories as biased against their point of view (and/or in favor of someone else's point of view) has been termed the hostile media effect.5 Studies of the hostile media effect (HME) are informed by two classic studies of biased assimilation of information, which have shown that people with opposing views can read identical pieces of inconclusive evidence regarding an issue, yet conclude that the evidence favors their own existing opinions. One study6 found that two groups, differing in support for capital punishment and reading the same scientific statements on the efficacy of the death penalty in deterring future crime, each felt the data confirmed their prior attitudes and beliefs. Additionally, participants tended to discount the source of evidence when it was contrary to their initial beliefs and more strongly approved of the source when the evidence supported their initial beliefs. Similarly, another early study found that rival football fans who viewed the same close, penalty-ridden game came to radically different conclusions about how many infractions were assessed for each team.7

The tendency to interpret information in a manner that supports one's existing attitudes and beliefs is well documented.8 Vallone, Ross, and Lepper explain how selective perception contributes to news processing and subsequent judgments of the news media:

Partisans who have consistently processed facts and arguments in light of their preconceptions and prejudices (accepting information at face value, or subjecting it to harsh scrutiny, as a function of its congruence with these preconceptions and prejudices) are bound to believe that the preponderance of reliable, pertinent evidence favors their viewpoint. Accordingly, to the extent that the small sample of evidence and argument featured in a media presentation seems unrepresentative of this larger "population" of information, perceivers will charge bias in the presentation and will be likely to infer hostility and bias on the part of those responsible for it. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

An Experimental Investigation of News Source and the Hostile Media Effect
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.