Discrete Threshold versus Continuous Strength Models of Perceptual Recognition

By Paap, Kenneth R; Chun, Elvin et al. | Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, December 1999 | Go to article overview

Discrete Threshold versus Continuous Strength Models of Perceptual Recognition


Paap, Kenneth R, Chun, Elvin, Vonnahme, Paul, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology


Abstract Two experiments were designed to test discretethreshold models of letter and word recognition against models that assume that decision criteria are applied to measures of continuous strength. Although our goal is to adjudicate this matter with respect to broad classes of models, some of the specific predictions for discrete-threshold are generated from Grainger and Jacobs' (1994) DualReadout Model (DROM) and some of the predictions for continuous strength are generated from a revised version of the Activation-Verification Model (Paap, Newsome, McDonald, & Schvaneveldt, 1982). Experiment I uses a twoalternative forced-choice task that is followed by an assessment of confidence and then a whole report if a word is recognized. Factors are manipulated to assess the presence or magnitude of a neighbourhood-frequency effect, a lexicalbias effect, a word-superiority effect, and a pseudoword advantage. Several discrepancies between DROM's predictions and the obtained data are noted. Both types of models were also used to predict the distribution of responses across the levels of confidence for each individual participant. The predictions based on continuous strength were superior. Experiment 2 used a same-different task and confidence ratings to enable the generation of receiver operating characteristics (Rocs). The shapes of the ROCs are more consistent with the continuous strength assumption than with a discrete threshold.

Researchers working in the domain of visual word recognition have played an important role in advancing and establishing techniques for cognitive modeling. The early information processing era held that the language-processing system was highly modular and that an appropriate notation for representing the hypothesized components of such systems was the box and arrow notation. Deservedly, one typically points to Morton's (1969, 1970, 1979, 1982) logogen model as the seminal example of this approach. Influential shoots and hybrids include models by Meyer and Schvaneveldt (1971), Massaro (1975), Forster (1976), Becker (1976), Coltheart (1978), Carr and Pollatsek (1985), Neely and Keefe (1989), Besner and Smith (1992), and Johnson and Pugh (1994).

An alternative nonmodular approach is based on dy@ namic systems theory (Carello, Turvey, & Lukatela, 1992; Grossberg & Stone, 1986; Lukatela, Frost, & Turvey, 1999; Stone & Van Orden, 1994; Van Orden & Goldinger, 1994; Van Orden, Pennington, & Stone, 1990). Although many of these models serve as powerful heuristics, other contemporary models make quantitative predictions based on mathematical equations or computer simulations (Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 1999; Grainger & Jacobs, 1994, 1996; Jacobs & Grainger, 1992; Kawamoto & Zemblidge,1992; Massaro & Friedman, 1990; Masson, 1995; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; Norris, 1994; Paap, Johansen, Chun, & Vonnahme, 1999; Paap, Newsome, McDonald, & Schvaneveldt, 1982; Plaut, McClelland, Seidenberg, & Patterson, 1996; Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989; Zorzi, Houghton, & Butterworth, 1998).

Our approach assumes that it is fruitful to cleave the large set of models into a small number of distinct classes. The goal of this study is to test one of the major distinctions among models of word recognition. More specifically, converging operations are used to determine if the decision mechanism for perceptual identification is best described in terms of continuous strength coupled with a response criterion, as a discrete threshold, or as a combination of both. Experiment I uses the distribution of confidence ratings in a two-alternative forced-choice task to evaluate the models and Experiment 2 uses the shape of the ROC function obtained in a same-different task.

THE DUAL-READOUT MODEL (DROM) AS A DISCRETE THRESHOLD MODEL

A prototype of a discrete threshold model is provided by Grainger and Jacobs' (1994) DROM. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Discrete Threshold versus Continuous Strength Models of Perceptual Recognition
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.