The Latest on Recovered Memory

By Butler, Katy | Family Therapy Networker, November/December 1996 | Go to article overview

The Latest on Recovered Memory


Butler, Katy, Family Therapy Networker


NO SCIENTIFIC QUESTION RAISED by memory's malleability and persistence has reverberated more deeply recently than that of "recovered memories." Can an accomplished writer really forget for years and then suddenly recall that she was molested by a fisherman, as Isabel Allende describes in her memoir Paula? Can outwardly normal people .suddenly remember repeated childhood molestations by an uncle, father or cousin as an Ohio dairy farmer's wife, a Seattle computer analyst and a Salt Lake City homemaker have claimed in civil trials? Can people really be led to fabricate elaborate memories of Satanic rituals or to falsely accuse their fathers, as women in Texas, California, Minnesota. Colorado and elsewhere have claimed in lawsuits against their therapists? And can science distinguish false memories from true?

In 1992, accused parents in the False Memory Syndrome Foundation (FMSF) challenged the psychology professions to come up with hard scientific proof that traumas could be forgotten and then remembered; they compared them to recollections of abduction by space aliens. Now a more complex picture of false and traumatic memory is emerging. The validity of a handful of recovered memories is now far better documented and so are some false memories. And science is still far from developing a litmus test to distinguish one from the other.

In the last four years, more than a dozen cases of well-corroborated recovered memory have surfaced, and the popular press has begun to back off from the notion that all recovered memories are bogus. As Newsweek science writer Sharon Begley wrote last July, "Just two years ago, some dogmatically asserted that traumatic memories are never lost and recovered memories are always fabricated. Now a more balanced viewpoint is emerging. Some victims of childhood abuse do forget single episodes and perhaps multiple incidents."

In the spring of 1992, for instance, John Robitaille. a Providence, R.I., communications consultant, heard a news report about the notorious pedophile priest Father James Porter, and suddenly recalled that he, too, had been his victim. His specific memories were confirmed by two classmates: Porter later pleaded guilty to molesting 28 of 153 reported victims. Harvard psychiatrist Stuart Grassian surveyed 43 of them in 1993 and found another 8 or 19 percent who reported no thoughts or memories of the childhood abuse until the case broke in the media. Others reported years of forgetting.

In 1994. Brown University professor Ross Chest marshaled five corroborating witnesses and a taped confession from a man who molested him 25 years earlier at a San Francisco Boys Chorus summer camp, winning an apology from the chorus and a settlement of $35,000. Jury awards in other recent cases have reached as high as S2.65 million.

"There are corroborated cases that I accept as validated," says Harvard University memory researcher Daniel Schacter, whose book Searching for Memory argues for the existence of both false and genuine recovered memories. "They tend to involve events that occurred some time ago, not yesterday, and single or small numbers of events. What's still missing are similarly well-described and corroborated accounts of cases where the forgetting is much more extensive such as years of much more horrific kinds of abuse."

Retractors have also come forward with well-validated accounts of suggestive, abusive therapy that led to bogus memories. More than 250 have contacted the False Memory Syndrome Foundation directly, and FMSF parents report that another 100 sons and daughters have privately retracted. (Others have reconciled without retracting.) In malpractice suits against therapists,  22 retractors have won judgments and legal settlements ranging from $120,000 to more than S5 million in claims against their therapists. The largest amounts were paid to clients of psychologist Judith Peterson of Houston, Texas, and psychiatrist Diane Humenansky of St. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Latest on Recovered Memory
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.