The Primate Working Memory Networks

By Constantinidis, Christos; Procyk, Emmanuel | Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, December 2004 | Go to article overview

The Primate Working Memory Networks


Constantinidis, Christos, Procyk, Emmanuel, Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience


Working memory has long been associated with the prefrontal cortex, since damage to this brain area can critically impair the ability to maintain and update mnemonic information. Anatomical and physiological evidence suggests, however, that the prefrontal cortex is part of a broader network of interconnected brain areas involved in working memory. These include the parietal and temporal association areas of the cerebral cortex, cingulate and limbic areas, and subcortical structures such as the mediodorsal thalamus and the basal ganglia. Neurophysiological studies in primates confirm the involvement of areas beyond the frontal lobe and illustrate that working memory involves parallel, distributed neuronal networks. In this article, we review the current understanding of the anatomical organization of networks mediating working memory and the neural correlates of memory manifested in each of their nodes. The neural mechanisms of memory maintenance and the integrative role of the prefrontal cortex are also discussed.

Working memory is the term commonly used for the ability to maintain information in memory over a time span of a few seconds. Working memory constitutes a core component of higher cognitive functions including language, problem solving, and reasoning (Baddeley, 1992). Lesion studies first localized working memory functions in the cortical surface of the frontal lobe (Jacobsen, 1936; Milner, 1963). Neurophysiological recordings subsequently provided a neural correlate of working memory in the sustained discharges of neurons that persisted even after the offset of brief stimuli that monkeys were trained to remember and recall (Fuster & Alexander, 1971). Individual neurons represent particular features and spatial locations so that the activity of the prefrontal population can encode a remembered stimulus. The part of the visual space in which stimulus appearance can produce sustained activation has been termed the neuron's memory field, analogous to the receptive field of neurons that respond to sensory stimulation (Funahashi, Bruce, & Goldman-Rakic, 1989).

Neurons with memory-related responses have since been reported in multiple brain regions-for example, the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortices, which are the end stages of the ventral and dorsal visual pathways, respectively (Andersen, Essick, & Siegel, 1987; Fuster & Jervey, 1981). The prefrontal cortex is reciprocally connected to these areas in a well organized and systematic fashion: The dorsal prefrontal cortex (areas 8 and 46) is interconnected with the posterior parietal cortex, whereas the ventral prefrontal cortex (areas 12 and 45) is linked to the inferior temporal cortex. On the basis of this organization, Patricia Goldman-Rakic (1988) proposed that working memory is mediated by the sustained activity of neurons in parallel, distributed cortical networks. More recently, imaging modalities (PET and fMRI) have verified the concurrent activation of multiple human brain areas during performance of cognitive tasks that engage working memory, confirming the findings of the monkey neurophysiological studies (Courtney, Ungerleider, Keil, & Haxby, 1997; Jonides et al., 1993; Ungerleider, Courtney, & Haxby, 1998).

Over the last decade, great progress has been made in understanding the organization of the working memory networks and the functional specialization of brain areas that constitute them. Here, we will provide an update of the anatomical and physiological details of the working memory networks, focusing on the brain of the rhesus monkey as the best studied model. The prefrontal cortex will be the point of origin for our review. We will examine the organization of the prefrontal cortex and its connections with the sensory pathways-namely, the ventral and dorsal visual pathways and the somatosensory, auditory, gustatory, and olfactory cortices. We will also examine the function of the medial temporal system and that of limbic and subcortical structures. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Primate Working Memory Networks
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.