Prioritization by Transients in Visual Search

By Belopolsky, Artem V.; Theeuwes, Jan et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, February 2005 | Go to article overview

Prioritization by Transients in Visual Search


Belopolsky, Artem V., Theeuwes, Jan, Kramer, Arthur F., Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


There is an ongoing debate as to whether prioritizing new objects over old objects (the so-called preview benefit) is the result of top-down inhibition of old objects (i.e., visual marking; Watson & Humphreys, 1997) or attentional allocation to new objects, presented with a luminance transient (Donk & Theeuwes, 2001). In the two experiments reported here, we tested whether prioritization by luminance transients alone can produce a subset-selective search similar to the preview effect. Subjects viewed multiobject displays while a subset of objects was briefly flashed. The subjects prioritized up to 14 flashed objects over at least 14 nonflashed objects. Since prioritization by luminance transients can produce a subset-selective search on its own, it may well play an important role in the preview benefit.

The visual system utilizes a variety of attentional mechanisms to prioritize the selection of relevant information in the environment. Recently, Watson and Humphreys (1997) showed that visual search can be significantly improved when the presentation of search objects is separated in time. If a subset of distractors is previewed for at least 400 msec before the search display, the previewed objects can be excluded from search. This phenomenon, termed the preview effect, appears to be fairly robust across a variety of conditions; it occurs with previewed subsets of up to 15 objects (Theeuwes, Kramer, & Atchley, 1998) and is not affected if search objects change color (Watson & Humphreys, 1997,2002). Watson, Humphreys, and colleagues (Watson & Humphreys, 1997, 2002) argued that the preview effect is a result of top-down inhibition of the previewed (old) objects, a mechanism that they termed visual marking.

Even though top-down inhibition represents the modal view of the mechanism underlying the preview benefit, recently Donk and Theeuwes (2001) proposed an alternative account. They suggested that the preview effect occurs as a result of the prioritization of new objects (Yantis & Jones, 1991). They showed that the preview effect critically depends on whether the new objects are accompanied by luminance increments. When the new objects were presented as isoluminant with the background, the preview effect was abolished.

Even though Donk and Theeuwes's (2001) account seems viable, several arguments in favor of the top-down inhibitory account of visual marking have been put forward. First, the preview effect seems to have a relatively long time course; old objects have to be previewed for at least 400 msec in order for the preview effect to emerge, and it reaches asymptote at about 1,000 msec. Second, the preview effect is reduced if subjects are engaged in a demanding secondary task during the preview (Olivers & Humphreys, 2002; Watson & Humphreys, 1997). This implies that inhibition is an active top-down process that can be disrupted by a secondary task. Third, probe detection studies have shown that the detection of a probe dot was the worst at the locations of old objects, suggesting active inhibition (Watson & Humphreys, 2000). Finally, it is the accepted view in the literature that only a limited number of luminance transients, about four, can be prioritized for search (Burkell & Pylyshyn, 1997; Yantis & Jones, 1991). In visual marking studies, it has been demonstrated that at least up to 15 new locations can be prioritized for selection (Donk & Theeuwes, 2001; Theeuwes et al, 1998).

Given the literature reviewed above, the fundamental question as to whether the preview benefit is the result of top-down inhibition of old objects or of attentional allocation to new objects, accompanied by luminance transients, is still undecided. The experiments we report here were designed to test whether prioritization by luminance transients alone could produce a subset-selective search similar to the one found in the preview paradigms. We designed a paradigm in which a search subset was defined solely on the basis of luminance transients and there was no preview of a subset of objects.

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Prioritization by Transients in Visual Search
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.