Wise, and Otherwise

By Baltes, Paul B. | Natural History, February 1992 | Go to article overview

Wise, and Otherwise


Baltes, Paul B., Natural History


Biologists tend to treat aging, almost by definition, as a phenomenon of decline. But the course of a human life is more than just the inevitable outcome of biology. Human development, and thus aging as a part of this process, reflects the influence of two streams of inheritance: genetic and cultural. Two human accomplishments illustrate this: the ability of most of us to read and write and the tremendous increase in longevity witnessed during this century as a result of better medicine and more healthful life styles. Our genes provide the potential for both accomplishments, but neither would have been possible without the cumulative gains of human culture.

During the last decade, at Berlin's Max Planck Institute for Human Development, my colleagues (especially Reinhold Kliegl, Jacqui Smith, and Ursula Staudinger) and I have been studying the gains and losses of the aging mind. We distinguish between two major types of intellectual function: mechanics (information processing) and pragmatics (factual knowledge and problem solving). We argue that biological conditions reign supreme in the mechanics of the mind and that decline with aging is therefore likely. In the area of cognitive pragmatics, however, the power of culture unfolds, offering at least the possibility of gains with age.

To use a computer term, the cognitive mechanics are the "hardware" of the mind--the input of sensory information, visual and motor memory, and simple processes of discrimination and categorization. How quickly and accurately, for example, can we make distinctions between colors or geometric forms? How well do we remember simple visual or auditory stimuli?

To measure how mental hardware may deteriorate with age, we test basic memory skills of young and older adults. We ask participants in our research to memorize a long list of words, say, thirty nouns: car, plane, house, chair, and so on. To make the. task more difficult, we require that they remember not only the words, but also the order in which they were given.

When words are presented once at a speed of about two seconds per word, most of us can remember a string of five to seven words without the help of a memory technique. Remembering longer lists is possible, however, with the help of a memory strategy known since ancient times: the method of loci. To use this technique, you first learn a fixed list of places, such as the Eiffel Tower, the Berlin Wall, the Louvre, the Great Wall of China, the Golden Gate Bridge. You need as many places as there are words to remember. Once acquired, the list is a kind of mental map that can be used time and time again.

The next step is to form a mental picture, connecting each word on the list with one of the locations. For example, if the first word were car and the first place on your list of locations were Eiffel Tower, you might come up with a mental picture of a Mercedes dangling from the top of the Eiffel Tower. Then you move on to the next word and connect it visually with the next place on your mental map. Later, when asked to recall the entire list of words, you revisit the locations, one by one, retrieve the mental picture for each place, and decode it into the word to be remembered. With enough practice, you can remember long lists of words this way, and all in the correct order. Some people reach into the hundreds.

Because the mental operations required by the method of loci are part of the mind's basic biological hardware, we expected that acquiring and using the method should be fairly simple. And indeed, this is so. Almost anyone can learn the method of loci and, after just a few practice sessions, be pretty good at it.

But what about performance in old age? A typical seventy-year-old, we learned, takes three to four times as long as someone in her twenties to find and remember a mental picture linking word and location. Older adults also make more mistakes. And most important, the losses with age seem to be irreversible. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Wise, and Otherwise
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.