Use of Spatial Context Is Restricted by Relative Position in Implicit Learning

By Endo, Nobutaka; Takeda, Yuji | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2005 | Go to article overview

Use of Spatial Context Is Restricted by Relative Position in Implicit Learning


Endo, Nobutaka, Takeda, Yuji, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


For inefficient search, target detection is faster for repeated than for regenerated layouts. This effect, called contextual cuing, was assumed to arise from implicit learning of local spatial relationships between targets and distractors. However, a more global influence from distractors far from the target has not been tested. In this study, the search field was divided into upper and lower halves containing a repeated and a regenerated configuration set, respectively. The positions of the two sets were or were not exchanged, meaning that their relative as well as their absolute positions were the same or different (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, the repeated set appeared alone in either the same or the other half of the screen (same or different absolute position). The contextual cuing effect remained when only absolute position was changed, but not when both absolute and relative positions were changed. These results suggest that contextual cuing depends on relative positional information.

Visual context, such as the spatial configuration of objects, is a critical factor for the guidance of attention to a target location (Biederman, 1972; Biederman, Mezzanotte, & Rabinowitz, 1982). For example, Biederman et al. (1982) showed that a target placed in an irregular location was harder to detect than a target placed in its typical location. Recently, Chun and his colleagues have developed a paradigm using a visual search task to examine how the visual context of the search display affects search performance. In a series of studies, they demonstrated that the association between a target location and the distractor configuration is encoded when the same display is presented repeatedly, and that the configuration can be used as a cue for the target location in latter blocks, even if observers are unaware that the configurations are repeated (Chun, 2000; Chun & Jiang, 1998,2003; Chun & Nakayama, 2000; Olson & Chun, 2002). This facilitatory effect is termed contextual cuing.

In the paradigm used by Chun and Jiang ( 1998), participants were asked to search for a target (e.g., T) among distractors (e.g., Ls). There were two conditions: the old and new conditions. In the old condition, the search array presented in the first block was repeatedly presented throughout the latter blocks (i.e., the locations of the target and distractors were fixed). In the new condition, only the target location in a given search array was fixed across the blocks, and the distractor locations were randomly changed in every block. The results showed that reaction times (RTs) became gradually faster in the old condition than they were in the new condition. Participants did not notice the repetition of the search arrays in the old condition, indicating that the visual context (i.e., the association between the target location and the distractor configuration) was implicitly learned and was used to search for a target.

How is visual context represented? Recently, some studies have investigated the nature of the representation of spatial configuration. Jiang and Wagner (2004, Experiment 2) demonstrated that contextual cuing could be observed even when the search display was rescaled from 18° × 12° of visual angle in the training phase to 22.5° × 15° (or to 14.4° × 9.6°) of visual angle in the test phase, suggesting that the visual context is encoded on the basis of the relative positions of the search items rather than of the absolute position of each item in the CRT display. This account is also supported by the results of a study using 3-D search arrays. Chua and Chun (2003) investigated whether contextual cuing was affected by increasing the angle of rotation in depth (0°, 15°, 30°, and 45°) away from the training views. They reported that contextual cuing for the 3-D configuration was observed in the training phase, but that the learning effect decreased and disappeared with increases in the rotation angle. This finding indicates that the representation of spatial configuration is viewpoint dependent, so that contextual cuing is weakened by warping the relative positions of the search items in the 2-D image. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Use of Spatial Context Is Restricted by Relative Position in Implicit Learning
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Welcome to the new Questia Reader

The Questia Reader has been updated to provide you with an even better online reading experience.  It is now 100% Responsive, which means you can read our books and articles on any sized device you wish.  All of your favorite tools like notes, highlights, and citations are still here, but the way you select text has been updated to be easier to use, especially on touchscreen devices.  Here's how:

1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
2. Click or tap the last word you want to select.

OK, got it!

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.