From Interpretation to Segmentation

By Koning, Arno; van Lier, Rob | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2005 | Go to article overview

From Interpretation to Segmentation


Koning, Arno, van Lier, Rob, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


In visual perception, part segmentation of an object is considered to be guided by image-based properties, such as occurrences of deep concavities in the outer contour. However, object-based properties can also provide information regarding segmentation. In this study, outer contours and interpretations of object configurations were manipulated to examine differences between image-based and object-based segmentation in a visual search task. We found that locating a two-dimensional object configuration with deep concavities in the outer contour depends on the type of outer contour of the surrounding distractors. In addition, locating a three-dimensional object configuration was harder when it was surrounded by object-based-disconnected distractors, as compared with object-based-connected distractors, regardless of image-based connections in these distractors. We conclude that segmentation based on the outer contours of a target facilitates its localization. However, when three-dimensional information is available, segmentation strongly depends on object-based properties, rather than on image-based properties.

In visual perception, segmentation is the process of dividing proximal stimulations into separate objects. Several approaches to segmentation that the visual system may take have been proposed. Among these are segmentation based on concavities in the outer contour (Hoffman & Richards, 1984; Hoffman & Singh, 1997), segmentation based on necks versus limbs (Siddiqi, Tresness, & Kimia, 1996), and segmentation based on the so-called shortcut rule (Singh, Seyranian, & Hoffman, 1999). However, the relative ease with which the visual system seems to generate interpretations of objects leads to the question of whether not only image-based (IB) properties, such as those employed in the approaches above, but also objectbased (OB) properties play a role in segmentation. In this study, we investigate the differential role of IB versus OB properties in the segmentation of objects.

Consider the objects presented in Figure 1. In Figure 1 A, the locations of segmentation based on, for example, concavities in the outer contour are indicated by the arrows. In this case, the segmentation process results in the perception of one object with two protrusions. Note that, any IB approach to segmentation would result in the same segmentation of the object as that presented in Figure IA. In Figure IB, the same outer contour is presented, but now inner junctions (i.e., the intersections of line segments that are not part of the outer contour) are also drawn. Again, the locations of segmentation based on the outer contours are indicated, as well as the locations of segmentation based on the inner junctions. Figure IB is readily perceived as showing an object with one protrusion and with a second object in front of it. Note that segmentation based on outer contours does not have to be incorrect when inner junctions are made visible. After all, on the basis of the outer contour in Figure IA, the likelihood of perceiving a single object with two protrusions is higher than the likelihood of perceiving two objects. That is, for two objects (as represented in Figure IB) to result in the outer contour presented in Figure IA, requires a highly accidental positioning of the objects. The example is given only to illustrate the fact that the interpretation of stimuli (on the basis of OB properties) can result in one's perceiving separate objects although the outer contour remains the same.

To investigate differential effects in the processing of visual features such as IB and OB properties, a visual search task can be used. In a visual search task, a target has to be detected among an increasing number of distractors (Treisman & Gelade, 1980). The target can, for example, differ from the distractors on the basis of a single stimulus property. When this target is detected equally quickly among an increasing number of distractors, the slope of search time as a function of display size will be low or, perhaps, close to zero. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

From Interpretation to Segmentation
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Full screen

matching results for page

Cited passage

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

"Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited passage

Thanks for trying Questia!

Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

Already a member? Log in now.