Viewer Aggression and Homophily, Identification, and Parasocial Relationships with Television Characters

By Eyal, Keren; Rubin, Alan M. | Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, March 2003 | Go to article overview

Viewer Aggression and Homophily, Identification, and Parasocial Relationships with Television Characters


Eyal, Keren, Rubin, Alan M., Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media


Since the early days of television, people have been concerned about the possible negative effects of television on violent or aggressive behavior. One research direction to studying these effects has been to explore people's perceptions of the characters they encounter on the programs they watch. Perceived homophily, identification, and parasocial relationships with these characters are important because they may "mediate short- and long-term emotional reactions to depicted events and to characters themselves" (Hoffner & Cantor, 1991, p. 64). Connections between these relationships and trait aggression, which might help explain viewer aggression, have not yet been tested. We sought to account for these relationships with aggressive characters as they are linked to one's level of trait aggression.

Researchers have found that the effects of encountering violent television content are not uniform, but are based on viewer characteristics (e.g., Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee, 1972). Among these characteristics is a disposition to behave aggressively. Aggressive dispositions are trait characteristics that develop over time and are stable across situations and time (Botha & Mels, 1990; Huesmann, Eron, Lefkowitz, & Waider, 1984). Huesmann and Eron (1986) noted that these tendencies predict serious adult antisocial behavior. Situational factors, such as familial and environmental background and observational learning conditions, interact with such aggressive tendencies to influence the probability of an aggressive response. In general, researchers have found support for the view that more aggressive people are influenced by exposure to media violence (e.g., Dorr & Kovaric, 1980).

Whereas people who are not aggressive in nature also may be influenced by media violence, the nature and extent of the effects are likely to be different from the effects on more aggressive viewers. For example, media violence may teach nonaggressive viewers aggressive attitudes, but it is likely to do this and more (e.g., reinforce existing aggressive attitudes) for aggressive persons. Violent media portrayals also might have nonaggressive outcomes, such as bringing about prosocial behaviors (Evans, 1989). Here, however, we were interested in aggressive outcomes because they are more problematic for promoting antisocial attitudes and behaviors, which are more likely to exist in aggressive individuals. We assessed viewers' self-reported aggression and their homophily, identification, and parasocial relationships with aggressive characters, which should mediate the content-effect relationship.

Some evidence for the importance of people's aggressive dispositions is provided by researchers who have noted that aggressive tendencies influence viewing preferences (e.g., Gunter, 1983, 1985). Gunter found that people with aggressive dispositions enjoy watching violent content, perceive violence in shows to be more humorous and exciting, and are more tolerant of others' violence than less aggressive people. Others have observed similar results about the influence of aggression on program preferences for children (e.g., Atkin, Greenberg, Korzenny, & McDermott, 1979; Cantor & Nathanson, 1997; Eron, 1963), for adolescents, and for adults (e.g., Diener & Defour, 1978; Heller & Polsky, 1976; Robinson & Bachman, 1972). Selective exposure provides one explanation of this effect (e.g., Zillmann & Bryant, 1985).

According to the selective exposure hypothesis, people selectively choose what they will be exposed to in the media. Although most studies have not focused on aggressive content, it is likely that selectivity influences the choice of such content, in that people prefer supportive rather than discrepant messages to validate their thoughts, feelings, or actions (Atkin et al., 1979). Social cognitive theory helps explain this selectivity in exposure to television content and the impact of viewing such content. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Viewer Aggression and Homophily, Identification, and Parasocial Relationships with Television Characters
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.