Blind Research: Are the Hard Sciences Immune from Experimenter Effects?

By Sheldrake, Rupert | Skeptic (Altadena, CA), Spring 2003 | Go to article overview

Blind Research: Are the Hard Sciences Immune from Experimenter Effects?


Sheldrake, Rupert, Skeptic (Altadena, CA)


IN SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH, AS IN EVERYDAY life, our beliefs and biases often influence how we observe and interpret the world. In experimental psychology and clinical research, this problem is widely recognized, which is why experiments in these subjects are often carried out under blind or double-blind conditions. There is solid experimental evidence that experimenters' attitudes and expectations can influence the outcome of experiments.

In single-blind experiments, an investigator does not know which samples or treatments are which. But when human subjects are involved, as in medicine and experimental psychology, double-blind procedures can be used to guard against the expectancy of both subjects and investigators. In a double-blind clinical trial, for example, some patients are given tablets of a drug and others are given similar-looking placebo tablets, pharmacologically inert. Neither clinicians nor patients know who gets what.

In such experiments, the largest placebo effects usually occur in trials in which both patients and physicians believe a powerful new treatment is being tested. The inert tablets tend to work like the treatment being studied, and can even induce its characteristic side-effects. Likewise, experimenter expectancy effects are well known in experimental psychology, and also show up in experiments on animal behavior.

It is fascinating to learn that blind assessment first began in the late eighteenth century, in which the first blind experiments were carried out to evaluate mesmerism. They were literally conducted with blindfolds, and took place in France at the house of Benjamin Franklin, the American minister plenipotentiary, who was head of a commission of inquiry appointed by King Louis XVI. (The report is translated and reprinted in its entirety in SKEPTIC, Vol. 4, No. 3.)

The use of blind assessment was adopted in the mid-nineteenth century by homeopaths, and by the end of that century was taken up by psychologists and psychical researchers. But it was not until the 1930s that blind techniques combined with no-treatment control groups started to be used in clinical trials, and only after World War II did blind assessment in randomized controlled trials became a standard technique.

In medicine and psychology, blind experimentation began as a deterrent against the unconventional, but its general importance is now recognized for orthodox research. It has been internalized within the mainstream. Although researchers in medicine and psychology have been aware of the effects of experimenters' expectations for decades, how widely has this awareness spread throughout the scientific community? Can the expectations of experimenters affect their results in other branches of science? No one seems to know. Most people simply assume that scientists in orthodox fields of inquiry are immune from the problem.

My colleagues and I have attempted to quantify attention to possible experimenter effects in different branches of science by means of two surveys. The first survey was of experimental papers recently published in leading scientific journals, including Nature and the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

In the physical sciences, we found no blind experiments in any of the 237 papers we reviewed. In the biological sciences, there were 7 blind experiments out of 914 (<1%); in psychology and animal behavior, 7 out of 143 (5%); and in the medical sciences, 55 out of 227 (24%). By far the highest proportion (but the smallest sample) was in parapsychology, 23 out of 27 (85%).

In the medical journals, out of the 55 reports involving blind methods, only 25% (11 of the total surveyed) involved double-blind trials. Thirty employed single-blind methods, with one or more of the investigators carrying out blind evaluations or analyses. The majority of the papers involved no blind methods. (The journals surveyed were the American Journal of Medicine, Annals of Internal Medicine, British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, British Medical Journal and New England Journal of Medicine. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Blind Research: Are the Hard Sciences Immune from Experimenter Effects?
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.