The Making of Television: Young Viewers' Developing Perceptions

By Tidhar, Chava E.; Lemish, Dafna | Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, September 2003 | Go to article overview

The Making of Television: Young Viewers' Developing Perceptions


Tidhar, Chava E., Lemish, Dafna, Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media


Many children around the world are born today into a media-rich environment. Multi-television homes, multiple channels, video recorders, video cameras, interactive video, and computers--all are part of their everyday life. Debates over the meaning and influences of this environment are common in current literature (Buckingham, 2000; Livingstone & Bovill, 2001). The present study sought to understand how the early experience of growing up in a media-rich environment affects development of young viewers' perceptions about television in general, and television visuals in particular.

The growing body of research on young children's understanding of television has revealed that by six years of age, children spend an average of two hours a day viewing a variety of television programs, including prime-time content (Comstock, 1991). They have favorite programs and tend to know when these are broadcast. They have a fairly good grasp of some audio-visual conventions (such as montage). They discriminate between children's programs and other television genres as well as between programs and commercials (Bryant & Anderson, 1983; Leibert & Sprafkin, 1988; Lemish, 1997). Yet, research anchored in cognitive-psychology perspectives suggests that young children's understanding of television content and television as a medium is different from that of adults (van Evra, 1990).

Young children's limited capacity to distinguish between fantasy and reality on television has been a central research theme and its potential implications discussed at length (Chandler, 1997; Davis-Messenger, 1997; Dorr, 1983; Fitch, Huston & Wright, 1993; Hawkins, 1977). These studies and reviews indicate that children use two principal types of criteria to make distinctions between fantasy and reality on television. Hodge and Tripp (1986) used the twin concepts of external versus internal criteria to describe them. By external criteria they refer to children's reliance on their knowledge of the real world as a function of their individual life experiences. Hawkin's (1977) well known concepts of television as a "magic window" to the world and of "social expectations" are but two examples of this type of criteria. Dorr (1983) further specified three levels of the external dimension. The first, typical of very young viewers' thinking, refers to the concreteness of the television image. "One may say that something on television is real and mean it is exactly as it is outside of television" (p. 202). This level is also referred to as "factuality" (Fitch, Huston, & Wright, 1993). According to Dorr's other two levels, children accept the fabricated nature of the television world and judge each specific content according to the criteria of possibility and probability: Are events, characters, messages, etc. as presented on television deemed possible in the real world? Or are they deemed representative of it and thus probably exist in reality?

The second group of criteria for judging television reality was defined as relating to the internal properties of the program, namely those related to the formal features of television. According to a line of research developed at the Center for Research on the Influences of Television on Children (CRITC) and summarized by Fitch, Huston, and Wright (1993), formal features are part of television's syntax and serve as cues to perceiving the reality of television content and to classifying programs. These features are intrinsic to the television text and play a part in viewers' interpretations. These interpretations are the result of specific visual and auditory production techniques and include both the specific and more global dimensions of the program's attributes (Huston & Wright, 1994).

The two interacting criteria--external and internal--can also be framed epistemologically (using Fitch, Huston, and Wright's 1993 definition) as "real world knowledge" versus "television related knowledge. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Making of Television: Young Viewers' Developing Perceptions
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.