Performance in a Complex Task and Breathing under Odor Exposure

By Danuser, Brigitta; Moser, Denise et al. | Human Factors, Winter 2003 | Go to article overview

Performance in a Complex Task and Breathing under Odor Exposure


Danuser, Brigitta, Moser, Denise, Vitale-Sethre, Tanja, Hirsig, Rene, Krueger, Helmut, Human Factors


INTRODUCTION

Chemical sensory stimuli are omnipresent in everyday life as well as during work. Very low levels of environmental chemicals can be detected and categorized as pleasant or unpleasant. Influences of odors on both human behavior and mental performance are phenomena that are generally accepted, despite the paucity of scientific proof. Investigations about odor effects on mental performance have mostly failed to show any direct effects. In a study with 5-year-old children (Epple & Herz, 1999), performance on a cognitive test in rooms with odors was similar to that in rooms without odors. However, the study showed that odors can become conditioned to experimental states and can have influences on behavior. Gilbert, Knasko, and Sabini (1997) tested the effect of ambient room-odor conditions on performance and mood and found no influence of odor conditions; odor suggestion, however, produced a significant sex-related interaction on a digit deletion task, irrespective of actual ambient odor presence.

Only Degel and Koster (1999) have shown that a room odorized with lavender had a positive effect on task performance and that one odorized with jasmine negatively affected performance. The arousal optimum concept (Yerkes-Dodson law; Yerkes & Dodson, 1908) was applied by Degel and Koster to explain why the arousing odor impaired performance whereas the more sedative odor facilitated performance. Only Knasko (1993) has investigated the effect of intermittent bursts of pleasant and unpleasant odors on task performance. No effects on performance were found, but participants who had been exposed to the malodors reported retrospectively that they thought the odors had a negative effect on task performance, mood, and perceived health. Neither of these studies evaluated physiological parameters.

Two main lines of arguments are found concerning performance decrements under environmental stimuli: One argues that such stimuli lead to an increase in arousal (activation) above an optimum, the other that such stimuli cause withdrawal of attention (Hockey 1997; Loeb, 1986). The arousal concept, which may be traced back to Sharpless and Jasper (1956), assumes that activation or arousal is dependent on the level of sensory input as well as the level of ongoing activity and that there is an optimum level of arousal below or above which performance will be inferior (Yerkes-Dodson law; Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). The distraction hypothesis holds that extraneous stimuli may elicit responses incompatible with task performance and so degrade it. Broadbent (1979) assumed that continuous, nonnovel stimuli will rail to distract. Therefore distraction relies on stimulus novelty or on a change of stimulation, whereas arousal increase above an optimum is more applicable to continuous stimulation.

In psychophysiological terms, moderate novel stimuli may induce an orienting response. Based on Sokolov (1963) and Lacey (1967), the orienting reflex is assumed to functionally influence perceptual thresholds in facilitating the processing of information about the external environment; it is therefore most plausible that stimuli that induce an orienting response have the potency to impair performance (Filion, Dawson, Schell, & Hazlett, 1991).

The conventional interpretation of Pavlov (1927) and Sokolov (1963) suggests that any perceived change in stimulation is sufficient to produce an orienting response, but this seems implausible in light of the great variability of the natural environment (Ben-Shakhar, Gati, Bassat, & Sniper, 2000). Bernstein (1969), Maltzman (1977), and Naatanen (1997) argued that a change in stimulus input may not be sufficient to induce an orienting response and proposed stimulus significance as an additional condition for the response. Gati and Ben-Shakhar (1990) assumed that both stimulus input and stimulus representations can be characterized by sets of features and that the assessment of each factor is carried out by a separate feature-matching process. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Performance in a Complex Task and Breathing under Odor Exposure
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.