Suggestibility and Children's False Admissions of Guilt

By McCarron, Michelle C. E. | Canadian Journal of Police and Security Services, June 2005 | Go to article overview

Suggestibility and Children's False Admissions of Guilt


McCarron, Michelle C. E., Canadian Journal of Police and Security Services


ARTICLE REVIEWED:

Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., Loyen, S., & Reyskens, H. (2005). "I hit the Shift-key and then the computer crashed": Children and false admissions. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1381-1387.

Although a confession is generally viewed as irrefutable evidence of one's guilt, research into the phenomenon of false confessions has revealed that, under a variety of circumstances, a questioner may elicit a false confession from a suspect. To date, the literature in this area has identified three specific types of false confessions. A voluntary false confession is one that is made of one's own accord, without any external pressure to do so. A coerced-compliant false confession, on the other hand, is when a suspect has been heavily pressured, often throughout a lengthy interrogation process, to confess guilt. In this case, although the suspect still believes in their innocence, he or she may confess as a way of avoiding other potentially negative consequences (such as a longer jail sentence should she or he be found guilty at trial) or to gain a reward. The third type of false confession, and one which has received much attention in recent years, is the coercedinternalised false confession. According to researchers, in this condition suspects may not only confess to crimes that they did not commit, but may come to believe that they actually did commit the crimes that they have confessed to.

During the past decade, researchers have begun to make a concentrated attempt to identify the internal and external factors that increase the likelihood of obtaining a false, internalised confession. Interview techniques such as isolating the suspect, sitting close to the suspect while questioning them, and conducting lengthy interviews, have been found to increase the likelihood of eliciting a false confession. Researchers have been particularly interested in the personality characteristics of the suspect that may contribute to the likelihood of making a false confession. One factor that has received widespread attention is suggestibility, defined as the extent to which people are likely to accept messages communicated to them through questioning. This factor has been identified as one that may play a significant role in eliciting false confessions from children.

In an effort to examine the role of suggestibility in eliciting false, internalised confessions from children, Candel, Merckelbach, Loyen, and Reyskens (2005) recruited 50 children aged six to nine completed a brief interview-style questionnaire and computer task. The goal of this study was to determine the extent to which children will falsely confess to causing a computer malfunction when accused by a researcher, and whether this relates to their level of suggestibility. Similar studies have been conducted with adult and adolescent participants, with the main difference being that older participants were presented with false evidence in addition to the accusation. In this study, the children were falsely accused, but no false evidence was presented. In order to accomplish this goal, each child attended two sessions with the researcher and answered a series of questions designed to measure suggestibility and to complete a computer task, during which the computer supposedly "crashed".

Specifically, during the first session, each child completed the Dutch version of the Bonn Test of Statement Suggestibility (BTSS-NL). This measure consists of a brief narrative accompanied by four pictures, a ten-minute filler task (drawing a picture), and a set of 27 questions of three types: questions containing leading or misleading information, which comprise the Yield scale; questions that are immediately repeated after the participant has given an answer, implying that the initial response was incorrect (the Shift scale); and simple memory questions included in order to disguise the nature of the task. Suggestibility was measured by the participants' responses. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • A full archive of books and articles related to this one
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Suggestibility and Children's False Admissions of Guilt
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

    Already a member? Log in now.