Instinct, Culture, and Cognitive Science

By Redmond, William H. | Journal of Economic Issues, June 2006 | Go to article overview

Instinct, Culture, and Cognitive Science


Redmond, William H., Journal of Economic Issues


Institutional economics in the United States is generally considered to have developed in and through two main eras. These eras correspond to differing assumptions concerning human nature, specifically with regard to the bases for institutional behavior. The first, associated with Thorstein Veblen, represents instinct psychology. The second, associated with Clarence Ayres, represents cultural learning. Recent advances in the cognitive sciences have furnished the groundwork for a different perspective on human nature, and it is emerging as a basis for understanding institutional behavior. This perspective highlights the evolved nature of the brain and views the mind as a suite of adaptive cognitive responses.

Of particular import for institutional analysis, the cognitive science approach does not displace insights into institutional behavior gleaned from the earlier instinct or culture frameworks. Instead, this approach incorporates both influences, albeit using somewhat different interpretations and terminology. What Veblen knew as instinct is roughly similar to what cognitive scientists know as modularity, which involves specific brain structures with specialized cognitive functions. The concept of modularity weighs against cultural determinism, in the particular sense of a tabloa rasa, or blank slate (Pinker 2002; Cosmides and Tooby 2002). However, it is quite compatible with the notion that certain modules have as their purpose the acquisition of specific cultural characteristics, such as language.

The paper outlines areas of compatibility between cognitive science research and aspects of the instinct-based institutional tradition as well as the culture-based institutional tradition. The paper also discusses the relationship between modularity, cultural transmission, and group-level selection in evolutionary processes.

Instinct

Instincts are genetically inherited proclivities which favor specific responses or behaviors. As such, they represent the biological realm. Like other genetically heritable traits, instincts were produced by variation and, if favorable, promoted through the process of natural selection. In the latter part of the nineteenth century, many scientists and philosophers held the view that significant portions of human behavior could be explained by instincts. This instinct psychology was influential in the thinking of Veblen, who used the conceptualization to account for the widespread presence of certain behaviors.

Along with the pragmatic philosophers, such as William James and John Dewey, Veblen sought to incorporate Darwinian understandings into human psychology and the social sciences. In so doing, Veblen's interest was drawn to commonalities, that is, features of human nature which appeared across societies. The assumption was that commonality on such a scale could only be accounted for by genetic inheritance, in other words, as a product of evolution. Veblen considered several instincts in his institutional analyses: emulation, acquisition, self-regard, workmanship, idle curiosity, parental bent, and a "habitual bent" which makes habituation possible (Jensen 1987; Hodgson 2004; Edgell 2001). These were not rigid and autonomic but were proclivities, subject to self-control and modification (Veblen 1914).

For present purposes, it is of interest to note that these instincts were conceived as being of a purely psychological character. That is, they were conceptualized as heritable influences on thought processes but without associated assumptions concerning specific structures of the brain. It is the centrality of brain structures which distinguishes the modern cognitive science approach from the earlier understanding. A growing number of scientists now believe that the human brain is not analogous to a general-purpose computer but rather is composed of specialized areas with dedicated functions. These are brain structures which have been produced through evolutionary processes and which serve to respond to long-standing adaptational problems (Cosmides and Tooby 1992). …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • A full archive of books and articles related to this one
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Instinct, Culture, and Cognitive Science
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

    Already a member? Log in now.