Issue Knowledge and Perceptions of Agreement in the 2004 Presidential General Election

By Kenski, Kate; Jamieson, Kathleen Hall | Presidential Studies Quarterly, June 2006 | Go to article overview

Issue Knowledge and Perceptions of Agreement in the 2004 Presidential General Election


Kenski, Kate, Jamieson, Kathleen Hall, Presidential Studies Quarterly


The importance of political information in the democratic process has long been debated. While some view political information as an indispensable component of a healthy democracy (Delli Carpini and Keeter 1996), others maintain that those with minimal political information can still use heuristics to make reasonable choices (Lupia and McCubbins 1998). In the context of this debate on the importance of political knowledge, we ask four questions about the 2004 presidential election: To what extent did voters in 2004 learn the presidential candidates' issue positions? Did those citizens with higher levels of political knowledge make different voting decisions from their less informed counterparts? On those issues where there was a mismatch between citizens' perceptions of agreement and actual agreement on the issues, could citizens have learned the candidates' actual issue positions from the presidential debates or the press? And, were candidate ambiguity and heuristic confusion possible culprits in the instances in which there was a mismatch?

An analysis of data from the National Annenberg Election Survey (NAES) shows that on the seven issue positions examined, citizen understanding of the presidential candidates' positions was relatively high compared to 2000. However, as in 2000, when people made mistakes in attributing issue positions to the candidates, the results helped Republican incumbent George W. Bush and hurt Democratic candidate John Kerry. Mistakes, in other words, were not randomly distributed between the candidates. Moreover, the more information citizens acquired, the more they tended to support Kerry over Bush, after controlling for six demographic variables as well as party identification and ideology. In short, political information matters. Content analyses indicate that citizens could have learned about the candidates' positions from the debates as well as press coverage.

Because U.S. citizens vote for candidates, not policy positions, understanding where the candidates stand on issues is important. Unsurprisingly, policy preferences influence, in part, how people vote (Page and Jones 1979). While the causal direction of the issue proximity and vote choice relationship has been the subject of some debate, it is clear that "issue proximities covary with voting behavior" (Brody and Page 1972, 457). When voters fail to see a difference between the candidates on an issue, the issue has no impact on the voting decision (Page and Brody 1972). Candidate ambiguity, therefore, inhibits policy voting.

Although voter knowledge levels are far from optimal (Delli Carpini and Keeter 1996; Waldman and Jamieson 2003), failures to detect differences between two candidates are not necessarily the fault of the voters. In addition to the limitations of humans' internal information-processing capacities (Miller 1956), four environmental factors may constrain citizens from perceiving issue differences between candidates. First, if the media environment does not provide significant coverage of an issue--either because reporters are not interested in the topic, cover it in ways that minimize learning (Cappella and Jamieson 1997), or do not see the issue featured in candidate communication-then media reports will not help voters see issue distinctions between the candidates. Second, if the candidates take clear stands on an issue but their stated issue positions seem similar to voters, then the issue will not receive prominent news treatment, and the similarity, even if noted in news and by voters, will not invite voter scrutiny. For example, Page and Brody (1972) explained the inability of the Vietnam War issue to drive voters toward a choice of either Richard Nixon or Hubert Humphrey as a byproduct of the similarity in these candidates' stated positions on Vietnam.

Third, if candidates agree on a policy goal (e.g., a prescription drug benefit) but disagree about the means of achieving it (e. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Issue Knowledge and Perceptions of Agreement in the 2004 Presidential General Election
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.