Typology and the Linguistic Macrohistory of Island Melanesia

By Donohue, Mark; Musgrave, Simon | Oceanic Linguistics, December 2007 | Go to article overview

Typology and the Linguistic Macrohistory of Island Melanesia


Donohue, Mark, Musgrave, Simon, Oceanic Linguistics


Recent years have seen much discussion on the use and meaning of typological argumentation when reconstructing language history and language relations. We address the conclusions and methodology of a paper "Structural phylogenetics and the reconstruction of ancient language history" (Science, Sept. 23, 2005), which claims that, on the basis of a typological comparison, the non-Austronesian languages and (Austronesian) Oceanic spoken to the immediate east of New Guinea can be shown to belong to two unrelated genetic entities. We argue that the data and discussion in this paper do not allow us to conclude that the non-Austronesian languages in the study form a valid linguistic group in any historical sense, or that the methods they apply can be used to make claims about linguistic relatedness.

**********

1. TYPOLOGY, STATISTICS, AND HISTORICAL LINGUISTICS. In this paper we intend to provide a qualitative critique of a recent well-publicized paper dealing with the historical interaction of Oceanic languages and non-Austronesian languages spoken in the east of mainland New Guinea and the islands extending east to the middle Solomons, an area its authors call "Island Melanesia." (1) The paper, Dunn et al. (2005), makes the claim that structural ("typological') features of a group of languages can be used to determine the genetic affiliations of those languages. This work has been popularized both within and without the linguistic community, and has been heavily cited as an example of the introduction of new (with the unspoken assumption: better) methods into historical linguistics. We show that the work in this paper does not establish this claim, and, moreover, does not establish any firm factual claims concerning their study area either.

Dunn et al. (2005) apply a computational tool used in (biological) genetic classification to language data, using a dataset based on structural features of language rather than one containing comparative lexical data. (2) The authors apply this method to the question of the relationships between non-Austronesian languages in Island Melanesia. The Oceanic population arrived in this region approximately 3,500 years ago (e.g., Lynch, Ross, and Crowley 2002; Pawley forthcoming), and the relationships between these languages are therefore accessible to the methods of comparative linguistics. These relationships have indeed been studied, and the existence of an Oceanic subgroup within Austronesian is beyond question; further, there is general agreement on the subgrouping within Oceanic (Lynch, Ross, and Crowley 2002). However, it has proved impossible to establish any similar relationships between the non-Austronesian languages in the region, which share almost no lexical cognates (see, e.g., Ross, 2001). As Dunn et al. note, there is archaeological evidence for human presence in Island Melanesia going back 35,000 years; if linguistic lineages are even a third as long as that, any relationships that might have existed in the lexical data would be expected to have been lost. As an alternative, Dunn et al. use structural data to try to recover relationships. Their dataset consists of 125 binary characters (relating to phonological inventories, morphological patterns, word order patterns, and other syntactic features) coded for 31 languages, 16 Oceanic and 15 non-Austronesian. As a first step, they carved out a computational analysis of the Oceanic data, and the result they present is a tree that agrees well with the consensus picture obtained from the application of "standard" comparative linguistics methods, as mentioned previously. Dunn et al. take this as evidence that historical relationships between languages can be detected using such typological data, and that they are therefore justified in applying the same technique to the non-Austronesian data. The tree that results from this second analysis defines three groups of languages that match the geography of the region. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Typology and the Linguistic Macrohistory of Island Melanesia
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.