Can Higher Grades Result in Fewer Friends? A Reexamination of the Relation between Academic and Social Competence

By Landsheer, Hans A.; Maassen, Gerard H. et al. | Adolescence, Spring 1998 | Go to article overview

Can Higher Grades Result in Fewer Friends? A Reexamination of the Relation between Academic and Social Competence


Landsheer, Hans A., Maassen, Gerard H., Bisschop, Paulien, Adema, Liesbeth, Adolescence


In contradiction to the assumption that social competence is independent of academic intelligence, significant positive correlations have been found between the two (Cauce, 1987; Keating, 1978; Walker & Foley, 1973). Cauce (1987) reported a correlation of .16 between grade point average (GPA) and peer judgments of popularity in a sample of secondary school students. Green et al. (1980) found a correlation of .33 between sociometric peer judgments of "best friend" and grade point average for primary school children. Pelligrini (1985) found a correlation of .38 between academic achievement and peer judgments of positive aspects of social conduct (such as leadership qualities) with primary school children. Fork and Tisak (1983) reported a positive correlation of .52 between peer judgments of social competence and grade point average with secondary school children.

A possible explanation for these positive correlations is that social and academic competence have global competence as a common element. Ford and Tisak (1983) have suggested that social behavior forms a separate domain, while cognitive social competencies have more similarities with academic competencies. Children who have better reasoning ability are better at both tuning their conduct in complex social situations and solving academic problems. Higher social competence could be explained by better cognitive abilities to choose social goals and to determine how to reach those goals (Ford, 1982).

The hypothesis of global competence contradicts the common notion that children with high learning capabilities are sometimes unpopular. Neisser (1976) states that "academically intelligent people do often behave stupidly. The existing evidence does not suggest that they are markedly more successful than the unintelligent" (p. 139). A possible explanation for a nonlinear relationship is offered by Coleman (1961): "Those who are seen as the intellectuals and who come to think of themselves in this way are not really those of highest intelligence, but are only the ones who are willing to work hard at a relatively unrewarded activity" (p. 265). Similarly, Hartup (1970) points out that "research shows that bright children who achieve at a realistic level have preferred status in the peer group; the overachieving child, however, may have a less certain position in his peer group" (p. 392).

Thus, there are two competing theories: the global-competence theory predicts a positive correlation between school grades and popularity, and the overachiever theory predicts a nonlinear relationship. Coleman's statement also suggests that the reward for a specific activity may play some role. It is assumed that efforts to achieve in some school subjects lead to lesser rewards than efforts in other subjects. Efforts directed at more abstract and less attractive school subjects may lead to decreased popularity, while efforts directed at subjects with everyday relevance, such as learning languages (e.g., English and Dutch), do not necessarily lead to decreased popularity.

METHOD

Subjects

Subjects were drawn from three secondary schools in Utrecht, a city in the Netherlands. A total of 157 students (81 females and 76 males, ages 16-17 years) in seven third-grade classes participated. The classes consisted of students of moderate academic level.

Instruments

End-of-year grade points for mathematics, physics, Dutch, and English were used as indicators of academic performance.

Social competence was measured by having each student judge whether classmates were more, the same, or less competent in making friends than he or she was. Social competence was calculated by averaging the responses. Earlier research (Landsheer, 1991) has shown that using at least three judges provides reasonable test-retest reliability (r = .79). Intergroup reliability is also adequate provided groups of at least three judges are used (r = .61). All students received the judgments of at least seven peers. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Can Higher Grades Result in Fewer Friends? A Reexamination of the Relation between Academic and Social Competence
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.